The Project Gutenberg eBook of Colour vision

This ebook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United States and most other parts of the world at no cost and with almost no restrictions whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or re-use it under the terms of the Project Gutenberg License included with this ebook or online at www.gutenberg.org. If you are not located in the United States, you will have to check the laws of the country where you are located before using this eBook.

Title: Colour vision

Being the Tyndall Lectures delivered in 1894 at the Royal Institution

Author: Sir William de Wiveleslie Abney

Release date: April 17, 2024 [eBook #73410]

Language: English

Original publication: New York: William Wood and Company, 1894

Credits: Chris Curnow and the Online Distributed Proofreading Team at https://www.pgdp.net (This file was produced from images generously made available by The Internet Archive)

*** START OF THE PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK COLOUR VISION ***

Transcriber’s Note

Larger versions of most illustrations may be seen by right-clicking them and selecting an option to view them separately, or by double-tapping and/or stretching them.

New original cover art included with this eBook is granted to the public domain. It includes part of the title page and an illustration taken from the original book.

Additional notes will be found near the end of this ebook.

COLOUR VISION

TYPES OF COLOUR VISION.

W. DE W. A., DEL.

W. GRIGGS, LITH.


COLOUR VISION

BEING

THE TYNDALL LECTURES
DELIVERED IN 1894
AT
THE ROYAL INSTITUTION

BY
CAPT. W. de W. ABNEY, C.B., D.C.L., F.R.S.,
LATE ROYAL ENGINEERS

WITH COLOURED PLATE AND NUMEROUS DIAGRAMS

NEW YORK
WILLIAM WOOD AND COMPANY


v

CONTENTS.

PAGE
Preface vii
CHAPTER I.
The Eye 1
CHAPTER II.
Simple Colours and their Mixture 15
CHAPTER III.
Three Colour Sensations Possible 32
CHAPTER IV.
The Young and Hering Theories of Colour Vision 41
CHAPTER V.
General Aspect of Colour Blindness 58
CHAPTER VI.
Colour Blindness exhibited by Colour Discs and exhibited by Luminosity Curves of the Spectrum 74
CHAPTER VII.
Luminosity of Colours to Different Parts of the Retina 88vi
CHAPTER VIII.
Luminosity of a Feeble Spectrum and the Limit of the Perception of Colour 98
CHAPTER IX.
The Extinction of Light from the Spectrum 108
CHAPTER X.
The Extinction of the Perception of Light by the Colour Blind 122
CHAPTER XI.
Tobacco Blindness 137
CHAPTER XII.
Examples of Colour Blindness due to Disease 148
CHAPTER XIII.
The Holmgren Test for Colour Blindness 167
CHAPTER XIV.
The Spectrum Test for Colour Blindness 180
CHAPTER XV.
The Young and Hering Theories of Colour Vision Compared 187
Appendix 201
Index 229

vii

PREFACE.

The writer had for some years past, in conjunction with General Festing, and recently as Secretary and Member of the Colour Vision Committee of the Royal Society, carried out a series of investigations on colour vision, and selected that subject when he was invited, in 1894, to deliver the Tyndall Lectures at the Royal Institution.

The brief time allotted for these lectures—an hour on three successive Saturday afternoons—restricted the discussion of some aspects of the question, and confined its treatment in the main to those features most readily explicable by the physicist, and to bringing into notice the latest results which had been obtained from physical experiments. How far theviii writer has succeeded in the task which he then outlined it is for the reader to determine.

There was no intention in the first instance to publish these lectures. After their delivery, many persons expressed a desire that the information they contained should be rendered accessible to such as were interested in the theory of colour vision, and in deference to that desire the lecture-notes have been re-cast in book form. For the reader’s convenience the matter is now divided into chapters instead of into lectures, and a few additions have been made in the text to explain some of the experimental work to those who have not facilities for its repetition.

The writer has to acknowledge several debts of gratitude. First, to Mr. E. Nettleship, for his kindness in looking over the proofs, and making valuable suggestions whilst the work was passing through the press; and also, as will be seen throughout its pages, for many of the interesting cases of defective colour perception which have been examined by the somewhatix novel methods described. Next, the writer’s gratitude is due to Professor M. Foster for the permission he has given to use his admirable description of the Hering theory; and, lastly, to the Royal Society for the permission it accorded to use various diagrams which have served as illustrations to papers which have appeared in its “Philosophical Transactions” and “Proceedings.”


1

COLOUR VISION.


CHAPTER I.

I must commence this course by saying that I feel the honour that has been done me in asking me to undertake it, connected as it is with the name of Tyndall, whose recent removal from our midst has been deplored by all lovers of science, and by none more than by those who have had the privilege of listening to him at this Institution. It is my duty to speak on some subject of physics, and the subject I have chosen is Colour Vision. I hope it will not be considered inappropriate, since it was Thomas Young, the physicist, whose connection with this Institution is well known, who first propounded a really philosophical theory of the subject. Interesting as it may be to trace how old theories have failed and2 new ones have started, I feel that for those who, like myself, have but little time at command in which to address you, the historical side of this question must of necessity be treated incompletely.

Colour vision is a subject which enters into the domains both of physics and physiology, and it is thus difficult for any one individual to treat of it exhaustively unless he be a Helmholtz, who was as distinguished in the one branch of science as he was in the other. I am not a physiologist, and at the most, can only pretend to an elementary knowledge of the physiology of the eye, but I trust it is sufficient to prevent myself from falling into any grievous error. I shall try and show you, however, that the subject is capable of being made subordinate to physical methods of examination. I must necessarily commence by a very brief description of those parts of the eye in which it is supposed the seat of vision lies, but in terms which are not too technical. As to the mere optical properties of the eye I shall say but little, for they are not necessary for my purpose, although more particularly adapted to mathematical treatment than the other properties I have to describe.

The eye may be diagrammatically represented as in the figure which is supposed to be a horizontal3 section of it, the figure being reproduced from Professor Michael Foster’s Physiology.

Fig. 1.

Scl is the sclerotic coat. Ch the choroid coat, with CP the ciliary process. I is the body of the Iris. R is the retina or inner wall. PE the pigment epithelium or outer wall. L the lens held by the suspensory ligament sp.l. VH is the vitreous humour. ON the optic nerve, ox is the optic axis, in this case made to pass through the fovea centralis, f.c.

As far as the perception of colour is concerned, the principal part of the eye which is not distinctly optical—i.e. for the production of images—is the retina, and this it will be seen is in reality an outcrop of the brain, the connection between the two being the optic nerve. Owing to this connection, it is not easy to determine where the seat of colour perception is located; but for the purpose of physical investigation this is not of first-rate importance, nor does it affect the discussion of rival theories except in a minor degree. There are other subsidiary adjuncts in the eye to which, however, I must call attention, as they have a distinct bearing on the apparent intensity4 of some colours and of the hue that mixtures of others are perceived. The first is what is called the “macula lutea,” or yellow spot, a spot which it may be assumed exists in every eye. It is horizontally oval in form, and is situated in the very centre of the retina, embracing some 6° to 8° in angular measure. It has a brownish or yellowish tint, and the retina at this part is slightly depressed, being bounded by a slightly raised rim. In the centre of this area the retina becomes very thin, having a depression about 1/100 of an inch or ·3 millimetres in diameter, which is named the “fovea centralis,” where it is said that vision is the most acute. This statement can be well credited when we come to consider where the seat of the stimulation of sensation lies. The colour which tints the yellow spot is strongest at the crater-like rim, and fades away centrally and peripherally, and is said to be wholly absent in the fovea centralis.

As the colour of this spot is yellow or brown in the living eye (and that it is probably brown the absorption indicates), it follows that white light passing through it must be deprived of some of its components, though in differing degrees. If the seat of sensation is at the outer layer of the retina, as we5 shall shortly see must be the case, it will further be seen that when light of any colour which the brown pigment will absorb more or less completely falls on different parts of the oval area, the absorption must vary at each part, and the intensity of the perceived light will be least at the rim and increase centrally and peripherally. As the centre of the yellow spot or fovea is coincident approximately with the point where the axis of the eye cuts the retina, the image of an evenly illuminated object, when looked at directly, must fall on the yellow spot. If, therefore, a patch of such light, the image of which more than covers the spot, be observed, it ought to exhibit a varying brightness of colour corresponding to the strength of the colouring matter which exists at the different parts. This it but rarely does, for habit and constant interpretation of what should be seen prevents the mind from distinguishing these variations; but if the colour brightness, as perceived by the different parts, be submitted to measurement by proper means, the variations in brightness of the image can be readily recognised. A very common method of exhibiting the presence of the pigment is to look at a bright white cloud through a layer of chrome alum. Chrome alum transmits red and blue-green rays. Now as6 the spectrum-blue rays are those which the pigment will absorb, it follows that the colour of the solution should appear ruddy to the central part of the eye, but on the rest of the retina it should appear of its ordinary purplish colour. At a first glance, and before the eye has become fatigued, this is the case, but the phenomenon soon disappears. Another way of forming an idea as to what the yellow spot absorbs is to throw a feeble spectrum on a white surface and cause the eye to travel along it. If the spectrum be viewed so that it does not occupy more than about 40° of the retina, the movement of the eye will show a dark band travelling along the green, blue, and violet regions as the image of these parts of the spectrum fall on the yellow spot, and their apparent brightness will increase as they fall outside the absorbing area. This proves that an absorption takes place in this area.

Fig. 2.

The retina consists essentially of an inner and outer wall, enclosing matter which is similar to the grey matter of the brain. On the inner wall are the vessels which are connected with the optic nerve. The outer wall is epithelium coloured with a pigment, and it is here that the visual impulses begin, although the rays of light giving rise to them have to pass through the thickness of the retina7 before so doing. It has already been stated that the light has to pass through the thickness of the yellow spot before a visual sensation is felt in the centre of the field, and the experiments just given offer a fair proof of the truth of the assertion, but there is still another which is perhaps more conclusive. Suppose we have a hollow reflecting ball, as shown in Fig. 2, and through an orifice A we project a beam of light to B, which meets an obstruction, S, in its path, then A B would be reflected from B to C on a screen C F, and the obstruction S would be marked at C. If another beam from D was directed so as to meet the same obstruction, its presence would be marked at F. Knowing the distance of the centre O of the hollow sphere from F C and its diameter, and measuring the distance between F and C and their respective distances from the axis of the sphere, the distances S B and S E can be calculated. This method is applied in the formation of what are known as Purkinje’s figures. The simplest case is where a beam of light is directed through the sclerotic and transmitted through the lens. Images of the retinal8 vessels are distinguished as at S, and it is found that they cast shadows, which are seen as dark lines in the glare of the field of vision. The sensation of light must therefore come from behind these vessels, and calculation shows that the seat of the sensation is close to the pigmented inner wall of the retina.

Lying here is a layer of what are known as rods and cones, which have a connection, either actual or functional, with the optic fibres which largely compose the inner wall of the retina, and are connected with the optic nerve. In the yellow spot the cones are much more numerous than the rods, but in the peripheral part the reverse is the case. In the fovea the rods appear to be altogether absent. The total number of cones in the eye has been calculated to be about 3,000,000, of which about 7,000 are in the small fovea. The number of cones will give an idea of their dimensions. This detail has been entered into as it has been supposed that these rods and cones are all-important in translating light-waves into visual impulses. The inner wall of the retina of most human eyes, as has been mentioned, is stained with a black pigment, fuscin, though in albinos it is absent. What its particular use may be is still unknown, for its change by9 light is so slow that it can scarcely be the cause of vision. In the outer parts of the rods is, however, diffused a substance highly sensitive to light, called the “visual purple,” from its colour, and a theory founded on chemical action, produced by a change in this substance, has been promulgated. Fascinating, however, as such a theory must be, it lacks confirmation. The fact that the cones do not contain it, and that in the fovea are cones alone, renders it difficult to reconcile the theory with the fact that this part of the retina possesses, we are told, the greatest acuteness of sensation as regards light and colour.

The eyes of most vertebrate animals, it may be remarked, have this visual purple, but in those of the bat, owl, hen, and some others the colouring matter seems to be absent. Visual purple is an interesting substance, however, and as it is found in the eye it probably exercises some useful function, though what that function may be is at present unknown. That images of objects can be formed on the retina, owing to the bleaching of this substance, has been proved by experiment. The purple is first changed to a yellow colour, and then passes into white. These “optograms,” as they are called, can10 be fixed in an excised eye if the retina be detached, and then be treated with a weak solution of alum.

Fig. 3.

Many persons are not aware of the extent of the field of view which the eye embraces. Vertically it takes in about 100°, whilst horizontally it will take in some 145°, more or less. The field is smaller on the nasal than on the temporal side. When both eyes are used, the combined field of view is larger horizontally, being about 180°. The field of view which is common to both eyes is roughly a circle of about 90°. There11 is, however, a marked difference in the distinctness with which objects are perceived in the different parts of field of view. On the fovea centralis two dots placed so as to subtend an angle of 60″ will be perceived as double. That is to say, if a piece of paper, on which are two dots 1/30 of an inch apart, be placed 10 feet away from the observer, these dots will be seen as separated, whilst dots (in this case they should be black and of good dimensions) placed half-an-inch apart would still appear as one if viewed at the same distance near the periphery of the retina. In the yellow spot the distance apart of the cones is such that they subtend about the same angle as the dots when they are seen separate, viz., about 60″; that is, they are about 16/100000 of an inch apart, and hence may have something to say to the limit of separation. The field for the perception of colour is different to that for light.

The diagrams (Fig. 3) will show the fields in a satisfactory manner. The concentric circles are supposed to be circles lying on the retina corresponding to parallels of latitude on a globe, and are not, therefore, equi-distant when seen in projection. To make these circles it must be imagined that we have a bowl, in the middle of which is a thin rod standing upright12 and passing through the centre, and another rod attached to it at the centre of the sphere of exactly the length of the radius. If this last arm be opened to make an angle of 5° with the fixed rod, and be twisted round like the leg of a compass against the bowl, it will make a circle, the projection of which will give the innermost circle of the diagram; if opened to 10° it will give the next circle, and so on for every subsequent 10°. The lines passing through the centre are 30° from one another, the line stretching from 360° to 180° being a line supposed to be vertical. By means of an instrument called the perimeter, the field of vision for each eye can be measured. With its aid any small object can be made to fall on any part of the retina by directing the axis of the eye to a fixed point and moving the object along one of the diameters. Suppose we wish to ascertain the field for a white object, a small white disc is moved, say, along the horizontal line, and the angles at which the retina just no longer sees it are noted. This gives two points in the field, and they are plotted on the chart—in Fig. 3 one touches the outside circle, and the other is at an angle of about 65°. The field of vision is next tested along another line, say 300° to 120°, and other points noted and marked on the chart. When13 the whole circle has been examined, the various points are joined together, and we have the boundary of vision for a white object. The boundaries of the colour perception for (say) small red and green discs are found in the same way. The former is depicted in the left-hand chart and gives the field for the right eye, and the latter with that for white in the right-hand chart for the same eye. It will be noticed that two boundaries are given, one taken at mid-day and the other at 6 p.m. The brighter the colour, the larger is the boundary in both cases, showing that the field of colour vision varies according to the illumination. Now it is difficult from this method of experimenting to determine whether the fields for different colours are the same or differ in extent, as we have no information as to whether the colours themselves which were used were physiologically equal. The only way by which this can be satisfactorily determined is by using spectrum colours each of known brightness and area. (Some preliminary experiments made by myself regarding the colour fields will be found in the appendix, and will be referred to later.) It must not be thought that the various colour boundaries mark the limit at which light is perceived, but only the limit at which colour is seen; outside the boundaries the objects appear of a nondescript colour,14 to which we shall by-and-by call attention. The yellow spot lies within the circle of 5°, and the blind spot on which no sensation of light is stimulated is shown by the black dot about 15° away from the centre.

I have only attempted to sketch, in unphysiological language, the primary apparatus with which our experiments in colour have perforce to be made.


15

CHAPTER II.

It will be seen, then, that in measuring colour or light several circumstances have to be taken into account. These are not simple, and require differentiating one from another before the results of colour measures can be finally laid down as correct, or as being held to be applicable to all cases.

We must naturally ask, what is colour? The answer I should like to pass over entirely. It can only be described as a sensation, just as we should describe touch as a sensation. It has, however, one advantage over most sensations, in that it is a sensation which can be submitted to empyric measurement. The question whether certain phenomena, such as the colours produced by simultaneous contrast, are subjective or real, does not require answering for the purpose that we have in view, but the results recorded may probably help to throw light on it. Colour is an impression caused by the stimulation in the eye16 of some apparatus, that lies near the outer wall of the retina, the effect of the stimulation being conveyed by the optic nerve to the brain. If this apparatus be complicated by being made up of distinct parts, each of which transmits its own kind of impression to the brain, it is not only quite possible, but more than probable, that when one part is absent or injured the particular impression for which it is responsible will be lacking, and that the sum of the impressions due to the remainder will be unlike that perceived when they are all working together.

In every investigation, whether it be in physical or in any other branch of science, it is better to work up from the simple to the more complicated; and acting on this plan, it is better to commence experimenting with simple rather than with complex colours, though they may apparently produce precisely the same sensations. I shall, with this in view, devote most of the remaining part of this chapter to some necessary experiments with simple colours. The simple colours are those of the spectrum, and are the result of motion in the ether, which pervades all space. The motion is in the form of undulations or waves, and each colour is due to a series of these waves, which have a definite length. Thus, 6562 ten-millionths of17 a millimetre produces to most of us a red colour in the spectrum (see Plate I.), occupying the position indicated by a black line known as the C line in the solar spectrum.

A table of wave-lengths of certain lines in the solar spectrum is given below:—

TABLE OF WAVE-LENGTHS IN TEN-MILLIONTHS OF A MILLIMETRE.

B, deep red 6866
Lithium, cherry red 6705
C, red 6562
D, orange 5892
E, green 5269
b, green 5183
F, bluish green 4861
Lithium, blue 4603
G, violet 4307
H, extreme violet 3968

The rays in the different parts of the spectrum being due to these simple vibratory motions, cannot be decomposed further. And it makes no matter whether we see them as different colours or not, they will always issue at the same angle from the same prism (if the prism be used to form the spectrum), when it is turned to the same angle to the incident light. Milestones are useful along a road to tell us where we are in reference to some central place, and these black lines in the spectrum serve the same end. But they have the advantage over the milestone, for whilst the last will tell us how far we are from, say, York or London, the former tell us our distance from18 a zero point. We thus have a scale of light of different wave-lengths laid down for us, which we can apply to the study of the sensations stimulated in the eye, and so have the means of instituting a comparison between the colour vision of different eyes. A mixed or composite colour is in a different category, however, to the simple colour, as you will see directly. It is one which may be formed by any number of rays of different wave-lengths falling on the eye. What these rays are we can only tell by analysing the light and referring them to the spectrum.

The instrument before you is one which I have used before in this theatre; but as the major part of my experiments have been carried out with it, in case those who are present may not be acquainted with it, it will be necessary to describe it very briefly. The general arrangement of the apparatus is given in the accompanying diagram, Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

R R are rays coming from the source of light, be it sun light or the electric light, and an image of the one or the other is formed by a lens L₁ on the slit S₁ of the collimator C. The parallel rays produced by the lens L₂ are partially refracted and partially reflected. The former pass through the prisms P₁, P₂, and are focussed to form a spectrum at D by a lens L₃. D is a19 movable screen in which is an aperture S₂, the width of which can be varied as desired. The rays are again collected by a lens L₄, and form a white image of the surface of the last prism on the screen E. If the light passing through S₂ is alone used, the image at E is20 formed of practically monochromatic light. Part of the rays falling on P₁ are, as just said, reflected, but as it and the refracted part are portions of the light passing through the slit S₁, they both must vary proportionally. If then we use the reflected portion as a comparison light to the spectrum colours, the relative intensities of the two, though they may vary intrinsically, will remain the same. The rays reflected from P₁ fall on G, a silver or glass mirror, and, by means of another lens L₅, also can be caused to form a white patch on the screen E, alongside the patch of colour. At M, or anywhere in the path of the beams, an electro-motor driving a sector with apertures which can be opened or closed whilst rotating, is placed, and the illumination of either beam can be altered at will. To obtain a large spectrum on the screen E, all that is necessary is to interpose a lens of fairly short focus in front of L₄, when a spectrum of great purity and brightness can be formed.

If it be required to measure the width of the slits S₂ (which we shall see further on is often necessary), a small lens of short focal length placed behind L₄ and near the slit will cast a magnified image on E, and by means of a scale placed there, the widths of each slit, if there are more than one, can be read off on the21 scale by bringing them successively into the same colour.

Fig. 5.

Originally the comparison light was a candle, and it answered its purpose fairly well, and for obtaining absolute measures is convenient at the present time. Fig. 5 will show its arrangement, but as both the candle and the electric light may vary independently of each other, it will be seen that for merely the comparison of the different spectrum colours, the previous arrangement is the better. In both cases the two beams—the direct and the comparison—may be made to cast shadows22 by placing a rod in their path, the shadow cast by one light is then illuminated by the other light. By moving the rod towards or from the screen the shadows can be brought side by side.

With this instrument it is easy to demonstrate that a mixed colour may be mistaken for a simple colour of the spectrum. In a glass cell with parallel sides is a solution of potassium bichromate, which, to myself and probably most of you, has a beautiful orange colour. The spectrum of white light is now on the screen, and if this orange liquid is placed in the path of the white light before it reaches the prisms, all the violet, blue, and most of the green is cut off, leaving some green-yellow, orange and red only on the screen. That these form the orange colour of the bichromate is readily shown by removing the auxiliary lens. The spectrum, which has its focus at D, is now recombined into a patch of light, which is at once seen to be the colour of the solution.

Fig. 6.

The colour of the bichromate is therefore a complex or mixed colour according to our definition, for it is made up of a large number of simple colours. What I desire to show, however, is that this complex colour can be mistaken by the eye for a simple colour. First, let us interpose the cell with the bichromate in the path23 of the reflected beam, and throw the patch of light formed by it on a white surface A (Fig. 6), alongside the patch of light B formed by the spectrum. Next let us pass a single aperture (Fig. 7), which can be opened and closed by a screw arrangement, through the spectrum. By careful movement we at length come to an orange ray, which is spread out by the apparatus to form a patch on B, that to the majority (and the word majority is used with intention) of people exactly matches the colour of the bichromate. Thus we have a proof that, as far as the eye is concerned, the simple and the complex colours are identical. This illustration of the want of power of the eye to analyse colour might be repeated as often as we like. We may pass coloured wools, for instance, through the length of the spectrum and show that they have the property of appearing bright in, and therefore of reflecting, some colours and of almost disappearing in others—a sure indication that these colours are mixed colours as they are made up of the rays which are reflected. Yet when viewed in white light they can in many cases be matched with simple24 colours in the way we matched the colour of the bichromate solution. This tells us that there is something which requires investigating as to the constitution of the perceiving apparatus, and points to the probability that it is less complicated than it would be were it able to differentiate, without the aid of the spectrum, between simple and complex colours. If the eye had a separate apparatus—and when I say apparatus I use the word for want of a better—for taking up the impression of every simple colour, it might well be assumed that a differentiation must take place.

There is one class of colours, it must be remembered, which can never be mistaken for simple colours. I refer to the purples—mixtures of red and blue—for there are no spectrum colours which unmixed can possibly match them. All other colours, as no doubt will soon be apparent, can be referred to some one spectrum colour, either in its pure state or else mixed with some variable quantity of white light. We are all familiar with the fact that there are three primary colours, and we are naturally led to consider these in the light of the experiments just made. As good a definition as any other of a primary colour is that it is a colour which cannot be formed by the mixture of any two or more25 colours. The original investigators in colour phenomena were the artists, and they found that neither red, nor yellow, nor blue could be formed by any mixture of pigments on their palette, but that all other colours could be made by a mixture of two or more of these three. Hence to these three were given the name of primary colours. When, however, the physicist began to work with the simple colours of the spectrum, it was speedily found that, at all events, the yellow was not a primary colour, as it could be formed by a mixture of green and red, whilst a green could not be formed by a mixture of any other two colours. This we can prove with our apparatus.

Fig. 7.

Three apertures, all of which can be opened or closed as required (see Fig. 7), are placed in the spectrum, one in the red, one in the green, and one in the violet. The last we shall not require at present, so it is entirely closed; but we vary the width of the other two. We find that with a little red added to a bright green, a yellow green is produced; with more red added we have yellow; with still more red, an orange. The relative brightness of26 the two colours mixed together can be shown by removing the lens which recombines the spectrum to form the patch of light. Each colour issues through its slit and forms its own patch on a white screen which, for the purpose, we make rather larger than usual. The two patches overlap in the middle (Fig. 8), and the pure colours are seen one on each side of the mixed colours.

Fig. 8.

Now, placing one slit in the yellow and another in the blue of the spectrum, we find that whatever width of slit we take, no green is produced, but that, in fact, a yellowish or a bluish white results, and that when the two slits are properly adjusted, a pure white is produced. Evidently since none of the intermediate spectrum colours between the blue and the yellow can be made by their mixture, certainly green cannot. Hence, with pure colours a green and not a yellow is one of the primaries.

Further investigation on these lines has placed the violet of the spectrum as a primary rather than the blue, but this is still a matter of debate. Suffice it to say that a red and a green in the spectrum are really two of the primary colours, and most probably the violet the third. Experiment shows that there is no other27 primary colour in the strict sense of the word. We thus arrive at the fact that, except the primary colours themselves, every colour in nature may be made by a mixture of two or three of these primaries.

Just a word of explanation as to why, with pigments, the primary colours appear to be red, yellow, and blue, and not red, green, and blue. The colour of a pigment, it must be recollected, is a complex one. If we analyse a yellow—a yellow glass will be just as good an example as anything else—we find it is made up of green, yellow, orange, and red. A blue is made up of blue and green. If a yellow is placed behind a blue glass, and we look at a white surface through them, the only light that can get through the glass is the green. If the light, coming through each glass separately, falls on the same spot on a white surface, it will be either colourless or bluish white, or yellowish white, whichever colour preponderates. As the light reflected from mixed pigments is made up principally by the light coming through the different particles, first coming through one and then through another, and only partially by mixed lights, it will be gathered why the primary colour, when deduced from experiments with pigments, was yellow, and not green.

With the spectrum colours there is this fact to28 remember, that though all intermediate colours between the pairs of primaries can be formed by their mixture, yet in some cases the resulting colours are slightly diluted with white, and that they thus appear less saturated than the spectrum colours themselves. The reason for this we shall be able to account for when we consider the colour sensations themselves.

When making matches to simple or other colours by the method of mixtures, we have to be careful of the conditions under which we experiment. This can be shown by a very simple experiment. I will make a match on B with the white light, which is thrown on the surface A (Fig. 6), by mixing the red, green, and violet that pass through the three adjustable apertures or slits already described. The apertures are altered till the match appears to myself perfect. From an appeal made to those of the audience who are at least 25 feet away from the patches of light, as to the correctness of the match, I gather that the match is to them imperfect. The mixed colours appear to them to give a pinkish white. The reason of this defect in the match is due to the fact that, as the lecturer is viewing the two square patches of 2 in. side from a distance of 2 ft. 6 in., their images on his retina extend beyond the boundary of the yellow spot, whilst the audience receives the whole29 of the image on that portion of the retina which is completely covered by it. To the lecturer only part of the blue and green is absorbed by the yellow spot, and the part of the retina outside it on which the image falls receives and records the full intensity of these colours. To the audience the full amount of absorption takes place, with the result that the patch of mixed colours must appear too red when it is correct to the lecturer. In this case habit makes the eye take an average of the different intensities which must exist at the various parts of the image. We can, however, cause a perfect agreement between all parties if the experimenter views the surfaces in a mirror placed some 12 feet away and then makes the match, for he is viewing the patches from what is practically a distance of 24 feet. If after making the match without the aid of the mirror the lecturer’s eyes are directed a little to one side of the illuminated surfaces, a match will no longer exist; the mixed colour, which is to the audience pinkish, will now appear a bluish green to him. The reason for this alteration in hue is that the whole of the images falls outside the yellow spot.

It will now be quite apparent that we must discount any assertion in regard to colour matches, unless we are told the distance of the eye from the surface on which30 the match is made, together with the size of that surface. This yellow spot is often provokingly tiresome in the study of colour mixtures, and one might almost be justified in doubting whether any absolutely exact matches can ever be vouched for, owing to the important region of the retina which it occupies.

The fatigue of the retina to colour after it has been presented to the eye for any length of time is a difficulty, but in a less degree. That the retina does experience fatigue can be shown by a very simple experiment. The lecture theatre is now illuminated by the incandescent light, and if we throw an image of the bright carbon points of the electric arc light on the screen and steadily fix the eyes on the image of the white-hot crater for some (say) twenty seconds, and then we suddenly withdraw it, a dark image of the points will be seen on the partially lighted screen, and will appear to travel with the eyes as they move away from the fixed point. This phenomenon is due to the fact that the perceiving apparatus for white light gets fatigued on the parts of the retina on which the bright image of the white carbon points thrown on the screen fell, and that when the source of brightness was removed, the less intense illumination of the screen failed to stimulate the vision apparatus at those parts to the31 same extent that they were stimulated over the rest of the field. We can vary the experiment by placing a red glass in front of the electric light, and, following the same course as before, we shall see a greenish-blue image of the carbon points upon the screen. In this case the retinal apparatus which has not been stimulated by the red sensation will be capable of the maximum stimulation by the feeble white light, whilst that part which has suffered fatigue will not respond so freely to the red contained in the white light. If we abstract a certain amount of red from the spectrum, its recombination will give a white tinged with greenish blue, which is a counterpart of the colour we feel when the eyes have been fatigued by the red light.


32

CHAPTER III.

Let me take you back again to matches of colour. We will now, however, make the matches with the primary colours in the guise of pigments. These colours themselves are complex colours, but as the eye cannot trace any difference, or at all events very little difference, between them and simple colours, a mixture of these complex colours should answer nearly as well as do mixtures of the simpler colours. We have here three discs, a red, a green, and a blue, and we can very closely match these colours by a red, a green, and a blue in the spectrum.

By having a radial slit cut to the centre of these card discs, we can slip one over the other so as to expose all three colours as sectors of a single disc. Then we can place the compounded disc on the axis of a rapidly rotating motor, and the colours will blend together, giving an uniform colour. Any proportions of the three colours can thus be mixed, and by a judicious33 alteration in them we now have them so arranged that they give a grey. By inter-locking together (Fig. 9) a black disc and a white disc, each with a diameter slightly larger than that of the other discs, but equal to each other, and rotating them on the same spindle behind the three colour discs, we can, by an alteration in the proportion of black to white, form a grey which will match that produced by the rotation of the three coloured sectors. In other words, white, though degraded in tone, can be produced by the three complex pigment colours, as we have seen can also be done by the mixture of the three simple spectrum colours.

Fig. 9.

The mixture of the three spectrum colours can match other colours than white. For instance, it can be made to match the colour of brown paper. By the colour discs also we can do exactly the same by introducing, if necessary, a small quantity of white or black, or both, to dilute the colour or to darken its tone.

Another application of the same principles enables us to produce an artificial spectrum by means of a red, a green, and a blue glass. By fixing these three glasses behind properly shaped apertures cut in a card disc at34 proper radial distances from the centre, and rotating the disc, we have upon the screen when light is passed through them a ring of rainbow colours. If the beam of light be first passed through a suitable rectangular aperture, the breadth of which is small compared with its length, placed close to the rotating disc, and an image of the aperture be focussed on the screen by a suitable lens, we shall have a very fair representation of the spectrum—every colour intermediate between the red and green, or the green and blue, being formed by mixtures of these pairs respectively.

We have now given a very fair proof that vision is really trichromic—that is, that it is unnecessary to have more than the sensations of three colours to produce the sensation of any of the others.

Fig. 10.

There is one colour, if it may be called so, that has not been shown you, and whether it is a simple colour or not cannot be stated. It seems, however, to be the basis of all other colours, since they all commence with it. It would, perhaps, be preferable to call it the first perception of light instead of a colour. We can exhibit this in a fairly easy manner by a little artifice. An incandescent lamp is before you, and a current from a battery passing through the carbon thread35 causes it to glow brightly. In the circuit, however, I have introduced what is known as a resistance, which consists of a very large number of square pieces of carbonized linen, pressed more or less tightly together. By means of a screw the pressure can be varied. When the pressure is somewhat relaxed, the resistance to the passage of the current is increased, and the carbon thread glows less brightly; and by a still greater release of pressure, the light can be made to disappear altogether. A beaker (Fig. 10) which we have here is covered with thin blotting paper, and when placed over the incandescent glow-lamp it appears as a luminous yellow cylinder, the colour being due to that of the light within it. We can next insert more resistance in the circuit, and it becomes red, due to the ruddy light of the thread. By inserting still more resistance into the circuit the red fades away, but in the darkness of this lecture theatre the beaker is still a luminous object, though faintly so. It has no colour, and the only sensation it provokes is one of light. Taking off the beaker, we see that the carbon thread is a dull red and nothing36 more. The passage of this light through the white blotting paper so reduces it that the red is non-existent, and the initial sensation is all we perceive.

Placing a piece of red, green, or blue gelatine round the lamp, we get the same effect, showing that the basis of all colour, be it red, green, or any other colour, is what appears to us to be colourless. This experiment is one which is full of interest, as it has a very distinct bearing on diagnosing our colour sensations, and a variation of it will have to be repeated under other conditions.

To go back, however, a little way, how does it arise that only three sensations are necessary to give the impression of all colours? One can understand that some definite period of the ether waves might be in unison with the possible swing of one apparatus in the eye, and another with another, but it is somewhat difficult at first sight to conceive that more than one can be made to answer to wave motion of a period with which it is out of tune, so to speak. A couple of illustrations taken from physical experiments may help to suggest how this can happen.

Fig. 11.

Fig. 11 is a double pendulum arranged as shown. The pendulum A is heavily weighted, whilst the pendulum B is light, being only a string with a small37 weight attached. This difference in weight was made designedly, to prevent any great effect of the movement of B being shown on A, though that of A must necessarily exercise a great influence on B. The two pendulums are now of the same length. A is set in motion, and as it swings, B also begins to swing, and soon is oscillating with greater motion than A, and continues to do so. The length of the pendulum B is next shortened, and A is again set in motion. B takes up the motion, and increases its swing more and more, but now the two pendulums are in opposite phases, and the motion of A tends to diminish the swing of B, and continues to do so till, after an interval of time, B is once more at rest, when it again will start swinging. The fact is, that when A commences to swing, B also commences; and as long as B and A are moving in the same direction the impulses tend to make B increase its swing, but when they are moving in the opposite direction, or rather, perhaps it should be said, when A begins to start from the highest point of its swing downwards whilst B is travelling upwards, the swing of B will gradually diminish. This, of course, must happen when B is38 shorter or longer than A, since their times of oscillation are then different. We can now picture to ourselves that when in the perceiving apparatus in the retina the moving parts—probably molecules or atoms—arrive at a certain amplitude, there is then an impression of light, and that it is quite possible that not only those waves whose motion is exactly of the same period as that of the apparatus will set them in motion, but also those waves which are actually of a very different period. If such be the case, it can be seen that waves of light of some periods may set each of the three kinds of perceiving apparatus in motion, and that possibly the resulting impressions given by the sum of all three for a wave out of tune with any of them may be even greater than when the wave period is absolutely the same as one of them. For in the last case a maximum effect may be produced on one apparatus, and the effects on the other two may be insignificant; whilst in the first case the effects on two of them may be so large that their combined effects may have a larger value.

Fig. 12.

The following diagram (Fig. 12), made on the principle of Lissajou’s figures, shows graphically the motion of the pendulum. The pendulum, with a pen attached, was started by an independent pendulum, which had a39 different period, and the amplitude of the former registered itself on paper which moved by clockwork round the axis of suspension. As the two pendulums had different periods, the amplitude, as shown by the traces made, first increased and then diminished till there was no motion, and then started again. The trace is very instructive, and deserves attention. It will be noticed that the amplitude, or length of swing, increased rapidly at first, and then very gradually attained a maximum. Having attained this maximum, the amplitude diminished very slowly for some time, and finally came rather rapidly to zero, and the pendulum for an instant was at rest.

Fig. 13.

The top figure is the red sensation on the Young theory; the middle is the green sensation, and the lowest the violet or blue sensation.

With the notion in our minds that the perceiving apparatus might act in the way that the pendulum acts, we naturally apply it to the theories which early investigators on colour vision propounded. Thomas Young, whose name has already been mentioned, had propounded a theory of vision, which depended on the existence of only three colour sensations, and Von Helmholtz adopted it and explained the action of the three sensations in reference to the spectrum as40 shown in the diagram. These figures do not pretend to be absolute measures of the sensations, but only of the form which they might take (Fig. 13). The height of the curve at each part of the spectrum is supposed to represent the stimulation given to each apparatus by the different colours. Looking at the figures we see that each sensation has a place of maximum stimulation, and that the stimulation falls off more or less rapidly on each side of this maximum. It will, however, be noticed that whilst the green sensation takes very much the form of the pendulum amplitudes (Fig. 12) between its periods of rest, the other two differ from it. In the case of the red sensation, the stimulation falls very rapidly in the red as it reaches the limit of visibility of the spectrum, and in that of the blue sensation the steep descent is towards the extreme violet. When the three sensation theory is examined in the light of the careful measurements that have been made, the results tell us that these diagrams can only be taken as suggestive.


41

CHAPTER IV.

An independent investigator of this subject was Clerk Maxwell, who experimented with a “colour-box” of his own design, by which he mixed the simple colours of the spectrum, and the results he got are really the first which are founded on measurement. He measured something, but hardly arrived at the colour sensation. His colour-box took two forms, both on the same principles, so only one will be here described, the diagram and description being taken from his classic paper in the Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society for 1860.

“The experimental method which I have used consists in forming a combination of three colours belonging to different portions of the spectrum, the quantity of each being so adjusted that the mixture shall be white, and equal in intensity to a given white. Fig. 14 represents the instrument for making the observations. It consists of two tubes, or long boxes42 of deal, of rectangular section, joined together at an angle of about 100°.

Fig. 14.

Maxwell’s colour-box.

“The part A K is about five feet long, seven inches broad, and four deep; K N is about two feet long, five inches broad, and four deep; B D is a partition parallel to the side of the long box. The whole of the inside of the instrument is painted black, and the only openings are at the end A C, and at E. At the angle there is a lid, which is opened when the optical parts have to be adjusted or cleaned.

“At E is a fine vertical slit, L is a lens; at P there are two equilateral prisms. The slit E, the lens L, and the prisms P are so adjusted, that when light is admitted at E, a pure spectrum is formed at A B, the extremity of the long box. A mirror at M is also adjusted so as to reflect the light from E, along the narrow compartment of the box to B C.

“At A B is a rectangular frame of brass, having a rectangular aperture of six inches by one. On this43 frame are placed six brass sliders, X Y Z. Each of these carries a knife-edge of brass in the plane of the surface of the frame.

“These six movable knife-edges form three slits, X Y Z, which may be so adjusted as to coincide with any three portions of the pure spectrum formed by light from E. The intervals behind the sliders are closed by hinged shutters, which allow the sliders to move without letting light pass between them.

“The inner edge of the brass frame is graduated to twentieths of an inch, so that the position of any slit can be read off. The breadth of the slit is ascertained by means of a wedge-shaped piece of metal, six inches long, and tapering to a point from a width of half an inch. This is gently inserted into each slit, and the breadth is determined by the distance to which it enters, the divisions on the wedge corresponding to the 200th of an inch difference in breadth, so that the unit of breadth is ·005 inch.

“Now suppose light to enter at E, to pass through the lens, and to be refracted by the two prisms at P, a pure spectrum, showing Fraunhofer’s lines, is formed at A B, but only that part is allowed to pass which falls on the three slits, X Y Z. The rest is stopped by the shutters. Suppose that the portion falling on44 X belongs to the red part of the spectrum; then, of the white light entering at E, only the red will come through the slit X. If we were to admit red light at X, it would be refracted to E, by the principle in optics that the course of the ray may be reversed.

“If, instead of red light, we were to admit white light at X, still only red light would come to E; for all other light would be either more or less refracted, and would not reach the slit at E. Applying the eye at the slit E, we should see the prism P uniformly illuminated with red light, of the kind corresponding to the part of the spectrum which falls on the slit X, when white light is admitted at E.

“Let the slit Y correspond to another portion of the spectrum, say the green; then if white light is admitted at Y, the prism, as seen by an eye at E, will be uniformly illuminated with green light; and if white light be admitted at X and Y simultaneously, the colour seen at E will be a compound of red and green, the proportions depending on the breadth of the slits and the intensity of the light which enters them. The third slit Z, enables us to combine any three kinds of light in any given proportions, so that an eye at E shall see the face of the prism at P, uniformly illuminated with the colour resulting from45 the combination of the three. The position of these three rays in the spectrum is found by admitting the light at E, and comparing the position of the slits with the position of the principal fixed lines; and the breadth of the slits is determined by means of the wedges.

“At the same time, white light is admitted through B C to the mirror of black glass at M, whence it is reflected to E, past the edge of the prism at P, so that the eye at E sees through the lens a field consisting of two portions, separated by the edge of the prism; that on the left hand being compounded of three colours of the spectrum refracted by the prism, while that on the right hand is white light reflected from the mirror. By adjusting the slits properly, these two portions of the field may be made equal, both in colour and brightness, so that the edge of the prism becomes almost invisible.

“In making experiments, the instrument was placed on a table in a room moderately lighted, with the end A B turned towards a large board covered with white paper, and placed in the open air, so as to be uniformly illuminated by the sun. In this way the three slits and the mirror M were all illuminated with white light of the same intensity, and all were affected in the46 same ratio by any change of illumination; so that if the two halves of the field were rendered equal when the sun was under a cloud, they were found nearly correct when the sun again appeared. No experiments, however, were considered good unless the sun remained uniformly bright during the whole series of experiments.

“After each set of experiments light was admitted at E, and the position of the fixed lines D and F of the spectrum was read off on the scale at A B. It was found that after the instrument had been in use some time these positions were invariable, showing that the eye-hole, the prisms, and the scale might be considered as rigidly connected.”

Fig. 15.

With this instrument he made mixtures of three colours, to match with white. By shifting the slits into various positions and taking as his three standard colours a red near the C line, a green near E, and a blue between F and G (see frontispiece), he obtained a variety of matches, from which he formed equations. After eliminating, or rather reducing the errors to the most probable value by the method of least squares, he got from his matches with white a table of colour values in terms of the three standard colours, from which the diagram of the spectrum (Fig. 15) was made. (The47 heights of the dotted curves are derived from the widths of the slits, and the continuous curve is the sum of these heights.) Now what appears to be a properly chosen colour does not necessarily stimulate only one sensation. Indeed the probabilities are against it, except in the extreme red and extreme violet. If colours intermediate to the standard colours be matched by a mixture of the latter, we do not arrive at any solution of the amount of stimulation of each sensation, since the chosen standard colours themselves may be due to a stimulation of all three sensations. As a matter of fact, Clerk Maxwell chose colours which do not best represent the colour sensations. The red is too near the yellow, as is also the green.48 The blue should also be nearer the violet end of the spectrum than the position which he chose for it. We may take it, then, that except as a first approximation, Clerk Maxwell’s diagrams need not be seriously taken into account. The diagram itself shows that the colour sensations are not represented by the colours he chose. Supposing any one in whom the sensation of green is absent were examining the spectrum, there would, according to the diagram, be no light visible at the green at E. Anticipating for a moment what we shall deal with in detail shortly, it may be stated that in cases where it is proved that a green sensation is absent, there is no position in any part of the spectrum where there is an absence of light. Had he chosen any other green, the same criticism would have been valid. The diagram as it stands is really a diagram of colour mixtures in terms of three arbitrarily chosen colours, and not of colour sensations. It merely indicates what proportions were needed of the three colours, which he took as standards, to match the intermediate spectrum colours. The negative sign in some of the equations—given in the appendix, page 201—may be somewhat puzzling to those who have not made colour matches, but not to those who have actually made experiments. It means that where it49 is present no match of colour by a mixture of the standard colours is possible; and that it would be only possible if a certain quantity of the colour to which is attached a negative sign were to be abstracted—an impossible condition to fulfil, but one which may often occur in colour-matching experiments. Later you will find that when colours are chosen as standards so that the resulting equations give no negative sign for any colour, we have a criterion as to the colours which give the nearest approach to the true sensations. The next diagram (Fig. 16) of colour sensations is due to Kœnig, who investigated the subject with Von Helmholtz. By a modified method, which perhaps need not be explained in detail here, he produced them, and they must be apparently not far from the actual50 state of things, supposing this theory be proved to be true. For my own part, I am under the impression that the positions of the colours which most nearly approach the colour sensations might be slightly altered in regard to the green and the blue, for reasons that will subsequently be given when the later experiments of General Festing and myself come to be described. For the immediate purpose of the lecture, the curves are sufficiently accurate, and I will ask you to notice what they tell us. It is presupposed in these diagrams that, if the three colour-perceiving apparatus are equally stimulated, a sensation of white will be produced; and the reverse, of course, is true, in that white will give rise to equal stimulation of the three apparatus. It follows, then, that in the parts of the spectrum where all three curves of sensation are seen to take a part in the production of a colour, such as at the E line, the colour is really due to the extra stimulation of one or two of the apparatus above that required to produce a certain amount of white. The colour in every part of the spectrum may be represented by not more than two sensations, with a proportion of white. In the orange and scarlet there are only two sensations excited, without any sensible amount of51 white, as the amount of violet sensation is extremely small. At the extreme ends of the spectrum only one sensation—the red or the violet—is excited; but in the region of the green the colour must be largely diluted with the sensation of white. As an example, we may take the part of the spectrum where the red and the violet sensation curves cut each other. At this point the green sensation curve rises higher than the intersection of the other curves. The red and the violet sensations have only to be mixed with an equal amount of the green sensation to make white, so that the height of the green sensation curve above the point of intersection represents the amount of pure green sensation which is stimulated. The colour is therefore caused by the green sensation, largely diluted with white. A scrutiny of the curves will show that at no point is the green sensation so free from any other as at this point, if we regard white by itself as a neutral colour. Looking at these figures, we can readily see what effect the removal of any one or two of the three sensations would have upon the colour vision of the individual. The probabilities, however, against two of the three sensations being absent must evidently be very much smaller than that there should be an absence of only one of the sensations, either red, green, or violet.

52

Fig. 16.

It will be well that we should also have before us the theory which is the only serious rival to that of Young, viz., that of Hering. In the report of the Colour Vision Committee there is an excellent description of this theory. As it was furnished by Dr. Michael Foster, we may be sure that the ideas of its originator are correctly given, and therefore I will quote it in his words:—

“Another theory, that of Hering, starts from the observation that when we examine our own sensations of light we find that certain of these seem to be quite distinct in nature from each other, so that each is something sui generis, whereas we easily recognise all other colour sensations as various mixtures of these. Thus, the sensation of red and the sensation of yellow are to us quite distinct; we do not recognise anything common to the two, but orange is obviously a mixture of red and yellow. Green and blue are equally distinct from each other and from red and yellow, but in violet and purple we recognise a mixture of red and blue. White again is quite distinct from all the colours in the narrower sense of that word, and black, which we must accept as a sensation, as an affection of consciousness, even if we regard it as the absence of sensation from the field of vision, is again distinct from everything else.53 Hence the sensations caused by different kinds of light or by the absence of light, which thus appear to us quite distinct, and which we may speak of as ‘native’ or ‘fundamental’ sensations, are white, black, red, yellow, green, blue. Each of these seems to us to have nothing in common with any of the others, whereas in all other colours we can recognise a mixture of two or more of these. This result of common experience suggests the idea that these fundamental sensations are the primary sensations, concerning which we are enquiring. And Hering’s theory attempts to reconcile, in some such way as follows, the various facts of colour vision with the supposition that we possess these six fundamental sensations. The six sensations readily fall into three pairs, the members of each pair having analogous relations to each other. In each pair the one colour is complementary to the other—white to black, red to green, and yellow to blue. Now, in the chemical changes undergone by living subjects, we may recognise two main phases, an upward constructive phase, in which matter previously not living becomes living, and a downward destructive phase, in which living matter breaks down into dead or less living matter. Adopting this view, we may, on the one hand, suppose that rays of light, differing in their wave-length,54 may affect the chemical changes of the visual substance in different ways, some promoting constructive changes (changes of assimilation), others promoting destructive changes (changes of dissimilation); and on the other hand, that the different changes in the visual substance may give rise to different sensations.

“We may, for instance, suppose that there exists in the retina a visual substance of such a kind that when rays of light of certain wave-lengths—the longer ones, for instance, of the red side of the spectrum—fall upon it, dissimilative changes are induced or encouraged, while assimilative changes are similarly promoted by the incidence of rays of other wave-lengths, the shorter ones of the blue side. But it must be remembered that in dealing with sensations it is difficult to determine what part of the apparatus causes them; we may accordingly extend the above view to the whole visual apparatus, central as well as peripheral, and suppose that when rays of a certain wave-length fall upon the retina, they in some way or other, in some part or other of the visual apparatus, induce or promote dissimilative changes, and so give rise to sensations of a certain kind, while rays of another wave-length similarly induce or promote assimilative changes, and so give rise to a sensation of a different kind.

55

“The hypothesis of Hering applies this view to the six fundamental sensations spoken of above, and supposes that each of the three pairs is the outcome of a particular set of dissimilative and assimilative changes. It supposes the existence of what we may call a red-green visual substance of such a nature that so long as dissimilative and assimilative changes are in equilibrium, we experience no sensation; but when dissimilative changes are increased, we experience a sensation of (fundamental) red, and when assimilative changes are increased, we experience a sensation of (fundamental) green.

“A similar yellow-blue visual substance is supposed to furnish, through dissimilative changes a yellow, through assimilative changes a blue sensation; and a white-black visual substance similarly provides for a dissimilative sensation of white and an assimilative sensation of black. The two members of each pair are therefore not only complementary but also antagonistic. Further, these substances are supposed to be of such a kind that while the white-black substance is influenced in the same way, though in different degrees, by rays along the whole range of the spectrum, the two other substances are differently influenced by rays of different wave-length. Thus, in the part of the spectrum which56 we call red, rays promote great dissimilative changes of the red-green substance with comparatively slight effect on the yellow-blue substance; hence our sensation of red.

Fig. 17.

“In that part of the spectrum which we call yellow, the rays effect great dissimilative changes of the yellow-blue substance; but their action on the red-green substance does not lead to an excess of either dissimilation or assimilation, this substance being neutral to them; hence our sensation of yellow. The green rays, again, promote assimilation of the red-green substance, leaving the assimilation of the yellow-blue substance equal to its dissimilation; and similarly blue rays cause assimilation of the yellow-blue substance, and leave the red-green substance neutral. Finally, at57 the extreme blue end of the spectrum, the rays once more provoke dissimilation of the red-green substance, and by adding red to blue give violet. When orange rays fall on the retina, there is an excess of dissimilation of both the red-green and the yellow-blue substance; when greenish-blue rays are perceived, there is an excess of assimilation of both these substances; and other intermediate hues correspond to various degrees of dissimilation or assimilation of the several visual substances. When all the rays together fall upon the retina, the red-green and yellow-blue substances remain in equilibrium, but the white-black substance undergoes great changes of dissimilation, and we say the light is white.”

It has been said by the same writer that this theory is tri-chromic. For my own part I do not see that it is so in the sense in which that word is used in the theory of Young. It may be a tetra-chromic, for as far as colour is concerned the black-white sensation must be excluded; but it appears to me that it cannot be strictly brought under the head of tri-chromic.


58

CHAPTER V.

The readiest means of investigating the stimulation of the different sensations necessary to produce colour is evidently by eyes in which one or two sensations are absent, and this applies not only to the Young theory, but also to that of Hering.

In former days, not much more than a century ago, the existence of colour blindness, as it is now named, was a matter of great curiosity, and in the Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of 1777, the case of a shoemaker named Harris is described by a Mr. Huddart, who travelled all the way from London to the Midlands on purpose to see if all the alleged facts regarding the patient were true. Harris mistook orange for green, brown he called black, and he was unable to distinguish between red fruits and the surrounding green leaves. At first, colour blindness was called Daltonism, from the fact that the great chemist Dalton suffered from it, and investigated59 the variation which he found existed in his vision from that of the majority of his fellow-creatures. It was in 1794 that Dalton described his own case of colour blindness. He was quite unaware of his defect till 1792, when he was convinced of its existence from his observations of a pink geranium by candle-light. “The flower,” he says, “was pink; but it appeared to me almost an exact sky-blue by day. In candle-light, however, it was astonishingly changed, not having any blue in it; but being what I call a red colour which forms a striking contrast to blue.” He goes on to remark that all his friends except his brother (mark this relationship), said: there was not any striking difference in the two colours by the two lights. He then investigated his case by the solar spectrum, and became convinced that instead of having the normal sensations, he only had two or at most three. These were yellow, blue, and perhaps purple. In yellow, he included the red, orange, yellow, and green of others, and his blue and purple coincided with theirs. He says, that “part of the image which others call red, appears to me little more than a shade or defect of light; after that, the orange, yellow and green seem one colour, which descends pretty uniformly from an intense and a rare yellow, making what I60 should call different shades of yellow. The difference between the green part and the blue part is very striking to my eye, they seem to be strongly contrasted. That between the blue and purple much less so. The purple appears to be blue much darkened and condensed.”

Dalton said a florid complexion looked blackish-blue on a white ground. Blood looked like bottle green, grass appeared very little different from red. A laurel leaf was a good match to a stick of sealing-wax. Colours appeared to him much the same by moonlight as they did by candle-light. By the electric light and lightning, they appeared as in day light. Some browns he called red, and others black.

Mr. Babbage, in Scientific London (1874), gives an account of Dalton’s presentation at Court.

Firstly, he was a Quaker, and would not wear a sword, which is an indispensable appendage to ordinary Court-dress. Secondly, the robe of a Doctor of Civil Laws was known to be objectionable on account of its colour—scarlet, being one forbidden by the Quakers. Luckily, it was recollected that Dalton was affected with that peculiar colour blindness which bore his name, and that as cherries and the leaves of a cherry-tree were to him of the same colour, the scarlet61 gown would present no extraordinary appearance. So perfect evidence was the colour blindness, that the most modest and simple of men, after having received the Doctor’s gown at Oxford, actually wore it for several days in happy unconsciousness of the effect he produced in the street. The rest of the description we need not reproduce. Both the above cases we shall see shortly come under the category of red-blindness in the Young theory. Recent investigations tell us that such colour blindness is by no means rare, nor can it have been then. Statistics, derived from carefully carried out examinations made in various parts of the world by an approved method of testing, show that about four out of every hundred males suffer from some deficiency in colour perception, but that so far as the more limited statistics regarding them are to be depended upon, only about four out of every 1000 women suffer in the same manner.

Colour blindness in a healthy subject is usually hereditary, and is always congenital. It is curious to trace back in some instances the colour blindness, where it is to be found, in a family. It often happens that colour blindness—as the gout is said to do—skips a generation. This is usually traced to the fact that the generation skipped is through the62 mother. Thus, the maternal grandfather may be colour blind, as may be the grandsons, but the mother will very frequently have perfectly normal vision for colour. On the other hand, the paternal grandfather may have defective colour perception, and this may be inherited both by the grandsons and the father. The remark made by Dalton regarding his brother’s eyesight points to the fact that his own colour blindness was probably hereditary. Deaf mutes, Jews and Quakers, seem to be more liable to colour blindness than other people, statistics giving them 13·7, 4·9, and 5·9 as the percentages. It may be well to point out that the deficiency in colour perception to which we are alluding is totally distinct from that which may arise from disease. This last form has such marked characteristics of its own that it can at once be distinguished from the congenital form.

Of the four per cent. of males who suffer from congenital colour deficiency of vision, a large number are not totally lacking in any one or more colour sensations. Those in which one sensation, on the Young theory, is entirely missing are called “completely red-, green-, or violet-blind,” whilst those in which the sensation is but partially deadened are called “partially red-, green-, or violet-blind.” When63 two sensations are entirely absent, and such cases are very rare indeed, they are generally said to have monochromatic vision; that is, every colour to them is the same, as is also white, the only distinction between any of them being the superior brightness of some over others. The best illustration of this form of colour vision is perhaps to say that the retinæ of such people have the same characteristics in regard to sensitiveness as has a photographic plate, the resulting prints in black and white representing what it sees in nature. When we have to adopt the terms used by the followers of Hering’s theory—the theory which obtains most followers amongst the physiologists, since it endeavours to explain colour vision in a physiological way, though it fails to meet all the requirements of the physicist—we should restrict our terms to red-green and yellow-blue blindness, still perhaps retaining the term monochromatic vision for the rare cases specified above. As we must employ some terms to express our meaning, we shall in these lectures adopt those of the Young theory.

Now taking a red-blind person and examining him with the spectrum, we find that he sees no light at all at the extreme limit of our red, and only when64 he comes to the part where the red lithium line marks a certain red does a glimmer commence; he then sees what he may call dark-green, or he may call dark-yellow. When questioned about what to us are greens he also calls them green or yellow, some being bright, others saturated hues, and others again paler. When he gets to the bluish-green he calls it grey, and will say it is indistinguishable from, and in fact will match with, a white degraded in tone. From this point he will say he sees blue, near F pale-blue, and in the violet dark-blue. Too much importance must not be attached to the nomenclature adopted by the colour blind. They have to take the names of the colours from the normal eyed. Yellow objects are generally brighter than red, and having annexed the idea that what to them is bright red is called yellow, they give it that distinguishing name. His limit of vision at the violet end will be the same as the majority of mankind, but it will be considerably shortened at the red end. The point in the spectrum which he calls grey is an important point, and corresponds to the place where the violet and green curves cut in Fig. 16. This point can be very accurately determined by placing alongside the colour patch A (Fig. 6) the white patch, which is reduced in brightness as required by65 rotating sectors. As the slit is moved along the spectrum it will eventually reach a point where he will say both patches of light are exactly similar in hue. To the normal eye one will be white and the other the kind of green indicated above (see frontispiece).

If a similar examination be made of the green-blind, the red end of the spectrum will be called red or yellow, but the spectrum itself will be visible between the same limits as it is to the person who has the normal sense of vision. A grey stripe will be seen in the spectrum, but in this case it will be a trifle nearer the red end of the spectrum than the point which the red-blind calls grey; from this point to the extreme violet, the green-blind will name the spectrum colours similarly to the red-blind. The part of the spectrum where grey exists to the green-blind is even more important than that part at which it exists to red-blind, for it marks the place where the red and violet curves cut each other in Fig. 16, and is in the majority of cases the place in the spectrum where to the normal eye the green sensation is unmixed with any sensation except that of white, as quite recently explained. This green evidently is the colour which is most usefully employed in making colour mixtures in order to obtain the three66 sensation curves of the Young theory, since white can be added to the colour matched. To avoid verbiage, we shall call the points where the red- or green-blind see a grey in the spectrum their neutral points, and the grey they see at those points their neutral colours. The three curves we shall call the red, green, or violet curves, and the slits, when placed in the red, green, or violet of the spectrum, as the red, green, and violet slits.

We have already mentioned the case of those who possess monochromatic vision, and shown in what respect they will differ in their description of the spectrum from those more common cases of defective vision. If the visual sensation they possess be the violet, they will see no light at the extreme red of the spectrum, and very little in the orange. They must match every colour with some shade of grey, for they will only perceive that sensation, in what to ordinary normal eyes is white. We need not detail how those who possess monochromatic vision due to some other sensation would describe the different colours. The diagram will tell us. Suffice it to say, that one colour will only differ from another and from white in brightness.

It is a very remarkable fact how many people who67 are defective in colour vision pass through a good part of their lives without being definitely aware of it. It is very doubtful whether, in the majority of cases, they themselves discover it. They may quite possibly attribute the descriptions of colour which they hear, and which appear to them absolutely false or meaningless, as due to mental or moral defects in their friends. I have had two cases of this recently. One was a gentleman of seventy-four, who had no conception that he had anything but normal colour vision; his daughters, however, had a suspicion that something was not quite right in it, and after a good deal of persuasion brought him to me to examine. The first mistake that he made was to state that he was sitting on a black velvet chair, whereas the seat was a deep crimson plush. He laughed at his daughter’s description of the mistake he made, and declared he was only colour ignorant, and that she was the one who was colour blind. The examination showed that colour ignorant he was, but that the ignorance was due to complete red-blindness. For the seventy-four years he had lived he was unaware of his deficiency, suspecting it in others, and it was only an accidental circumstance which made him acquainted with the true state of his colour perception.68 Another elderly gentleman, in a high position in life, was also accidentally tested, and he proved to be completely green-blind. He, too, was quite unaware of his defect, and protested that, yachtsman as he was, he would never mistake a ship’s lights; but a very brief test showed his friends who were with him that his declaration had to be received with a certain amount of reservation. Others there are who certainly do know that some peculiarity exists in their sense of colour, and, foolish as it may appear to be—though, after all, it is quite consistent with a sensitive nature—they have tried to hide their defect from their fellow-creatures. Such examples, no doubt, some of my audience have met with, and experience tells me that they have just as much reluctance to pass an hour in my darkened room as they would have to occupy a police cell. In those few cases that have come voluntarily to me for examination, the peculiarity in colour sense was first brought to notice by the patient—if patient I may call him—failing to distinguish between cherries and the cherry leaves, or strawberries and the strawberry leaves. Such mistakes committed publicly are usually the source of unbounded merriment and curiosity to schoolboys when made by their schoolfellows, and I69 am bound to say that even persons of graver years are not unapt to be amused at what they consider to be a shortcoming in their fellow-creatures. To the student of colour vision the discovery of curious cases of colour deficiency is looked upon in a very different light—a good case of colour blindness, or still better one of monochromatic vision, is eagerly sought after, with the hope of submitting it to a rigid examination. When we look at the diagram (Fig. 16) we shall find why it is that the colour blind describe the spectrum as they do. Literally for those whose vision is dichromic, it is made up of two sensations alone, and the colours to which these sensations give rise are mixed throughout a large part of the spectrum, the pure unmixed sensations being at each end of the spectrum as they are in normal colour vision. The annexed diagram (Fig. 18) gives the curves for a red-blind person as made by observations under Clerk Maxwell’s directions. The70 standard colours here have been badly selected, for one of them stimulates the two sensations possessed.

Fig. 18.

An easy and instructive experiment can be made to give an idea of the kind of colour that these colour blind imagine as white, whether they be red-, green-, or violet-blind. (For those who have only monochromatic vision, as before stated, white is coloured with the one colour they possess.) Three slits are now in the spectrum, one near the extreme end of the red, another well in the violet, and the third in that part of the spectrum in which the green-blind see their neutral colour (see page 66). With the three colours issuing from these apertures a match is made with the white patch, and in this case the match is made as seen from a distant point, so that the resulting deductions may be true to the audience. If a colour-blind person be in this theatre, he will agree with me that the match is as correct to him as it is to myself and the rest of you. So far we could not distinguish his colour perception from the normal, but if he be red-blind, and the red slit be covered, he will still say that the match holds good, for, as a matter of fact, the red with which we helped to build up the white is non-existent to him. The white that he now sees is to us the greenish-blue71 patch which the mixed violet and green make. If he be a green-blind person he will tell us the colour is a very pale blue, but when the green slit is covered up and the red uncovered, the match will once more be correct, though the purple, formed by the mixture of red and blue, will appear to him to be a little darker than the white. This is what one would expect, for you must recollect this green in the spectrum he would call white or grey. If then, from what to him is also white, though formed by the rays coming through the three slits, we take away a certain amount of degraded white (green to us), he must still see white, but darker. We have, however, met with what is an apparent paradox. The green, coming through the now covered slit, he calls white, as he also does the purple. To impress this point more strongly upon you, I will place in front of the green slit a small prism which has an angle of about one and a-half degrees. This is just sufficient to throw the green colour on the neighbouring white surface. Here we have both the colours which the green-blind calls white side by side. If the brightness of each be the same, he would see no difference in them. Is it possible that on any theory this can be correct? To explain72 this apparent paradox, and without reference to the mathematical proof that white subtracted from white leaves white, we have only to look at our diagram (Fig. 16), and it is immediately apparent how it arises. The red and the blue curves cut at this point; and if we take away the green sensation entirely, the residue will be a mixture of the red and blue, which is the identical purple colour forming the patch.

If we are wishful to ascertain the colour that the violet-blind calls white, we have only to cover up the violet slit and a yellow is left behind as the result. I would have you remark that these colours which are seen as white would only be of the hues shown you, supposing the colour sensations were identical with those in normal vision. Whether this is the case we cannot absolutely say, and the only way in which this can be authoritatively settled is by examining some person who has normal colour vision in one eye and defective colour sense, not due to disease, in the other. One such person has been examined abroad, but in what way I am unable to say. It is recorded that he sees the red end of the spectrum as yellow with the eye that is defective. Another person I have heard of in England, but so far have not73 had the good fortune to get hold of him for examination. When I can lay my hands on him, he will be able to help to confirm or disprove what should be a general rather than a particular case.

So far I have only met with what appears to be one genuine case of violet blindness. It is very remarkable, on account of the eccentricity of the colour nomenclature. The only two colours which the subject saw were red and black. He named all greens and blues as black, the distinction between the two being that the former was “bright black” and the latter “dark black.” Yellow he called white, and a glance at Fig. 16 will show that at this place in the spectrum the neutral point of a violet-blind should occur. By shifting the slit gradually into the green, he called it grey, instead of “bright black,” though it did not match the white patch when darkened. He called a green light a “bright black” light. We shall have to refer to this case when we are describing other investigations.


74

CHAPTER VI.

Another mode of exhibiting colour blindness, and one of the first adopted, is by making mixtures of colours with rapidly rotating colour discs. In my own experiments I have chosen a red, which is scarlet, over which a wash of carmine has been brushed. It has a dominant wave-length of 6300. The green is an emerald-green, and has a dominant wave-length of 5150. The blue is French ultra-marine, with a dominant wave-length of 4700. The card discs, of some 4 inches diameter, are coated with these colours as pastes, and by making an incision in them radially to the centre, as before described, and inter-locking them, the compound disc can be caused to show sectors of any angle that may be required. Outside these are the discs of black and white, the proportions of which can be altered at will.

The light thrown on the rotating sectors being75 that from an electric arc light, normal vision requires 118° of red, 146° of green, and 96° of blue to match a grey made up of 75 parts of white and 285 parts of black. For the last two numbers a correction has been made to allow for the small amount of white light reflected from the black surface. This correction has also been made in the subsequent matches which will be described. Colour mixtures such as these are conveniently put in the form of equations, and that given will then be shown as follows—

118 R + 146 G + 96 U = 75 W + 285 B.

(Here R, G, U, W, and B are used to indicate Red, Green, Blue, White, and Black.)

This match was exact also for all the colour blind, for the deficiency in one grey is also a deficiency in the other. With a red-blind, however, very different matches can be made, as the red pigment is a complex colour. There is in it, besides red, a certain amount of yellow, whilst in the green there is, besides green, a small amount of a red and a larger amount of yellow. The yellow will not only stimulate the green sensation, but also the red where it is present. Although in complete red-blindness the red sensation is totally absent, we may expect that a mixture of red and blue, as well as of green and76 blue, will enable a match to be made of the grey produced by the mixture of white and black.

This was the case. We have the following proportions—

295 R + 65 U = 45 W + 315 B.

When the green disc is substituted for the red, the red-blind made the following mixture—

229 G + 131 U = 120 W + 240 B.

It is worth noticing that the amount of blue in the first mixture is about half that required for the second. This tells us that the amount of green sensation stimulated in the first case is much less than in the second. As red can be substituted for green, it should follow that green, when rendered darker, should match the red. To try this a red disc replaced the black disc, and a black disc replaced the blue. The following match was then made—

131 G + 229 B = 340 R + 20 W.

It seems impossible to believe that these mixtures, so dissimilar in colour, could ever form a satisfactory match. This last equation might have been derived from the two first, in which case it would have stood—

137 G + 223 B = 342 R + 18 W.

By a completely green-blind the following mixtures were made—

77

251 R + 109 U = 62 W + 298 B,
and
277 G + 83 U = 107 W + 253 B.

In this case 363 Green are equivalent to 251 parts of Red mixed with 78 of White and 34 Black. The difference in the matches made by the two types of colour blindness is very evident. In the one case the amount of red required is much greater than the green, and in the other vice versâ. Another instance may be given of colour matches made, by means of discs, by a partially green-blind person, whose case will be more fully described when we treat of the luminosity of the spectrum to the different classes of colour vision.

His matches were as follows—1st, That of the normal vision. 2nd,—

160 R + 80 G + 120 U = 72 W + 288 B.

The green was then altered to 200, when the following made a match—

65 R + 200 G + 95 U = 72 W + 288 B.

Using these two equations, we have the following curious result—that 120 G was matched by 95 R + 25 U. As the green disc is nearly twice as luminous as the red to normal colour vision, this equation confirms the result otherwise obtained, that his78 blindness to colour is a deficiency in the green sensation. No mixtures of blue and red, or blue and green, would match a grey formed by the rotation of the black and white sectors.

I must now introduce to your notice a different method of experimenting with colour vision. If we throw the whole spectrum on the screen, and ask a person with normal vision to point out the brightest part, he will indicate the yellow, whilst a red-blind will say the green, and so on. This tells us that the various types of colour blind must see their spectrum colours with luminosity differing from that of the normal eye. The difference can be measured by causing both to express their sense of the brightness of the different parts of the spectrum in terms of white light, or of one another. Brightness and luminosity are here used synonymously. On the two small screens are a red and a green patch of monochromatic light—a look at the green shows that it is much brighter than the red. Rotating sectors, the apertures of which can be opened or closed at pleasure during rotation, are now placed in the path of the green ray. The apertures are made fairly small, and the green is now evidently dimmer than the red. When they are well open the green is once more brighter. Evidently at some time during79 the closing of the apertures there is one position in which the red and green must be of the same brightness, since the green passes through the stage of being too light to that of being too dark. By gradually diminishing the range of the “too open” to “too closed” apertures we arrive at the aperture where the two colours appear equally bright. The two patches will cease to wink at the operator, if we may use such an unscientific expression, when equality in brightness is established. This operation of equalising luminosities must be carried out quickly and without concentrated thought, for if an observer stops to think, a fancied equality of brightness may exist, which other properly carried out observations will show to be inexact. Now, instead of using two colours, we can throw on a white surface a white patch from the reflected beam, and a patch of the colour coming through the slit alongside and touching it. The white is evidently the brighter, and so the sectors are placed in this beam. The luminosity of (say) a red ray is first measured, and the white is found to require a certain sector aperture to secure a balance in brightness. We then place another spectrum colour in the place of the first, and measure off in degrees the brightness of this colour in terms of white light, and we proceed similarly80 for the others. Now how are we to prove that the measures for luminosity of the different colours are correct? Let us place three slits in the spectrum, and by altering the aperture of the slits make a mixture of the three rays so as to form white. The intensity of this white we can match with the white of the reflected beam. We can then measure the brightness (luminosity) of the three colours separately, and if our measures are correct there is primâ facie reason to suppose that they will together make up the brightness of the white. Without going through this experiment it may at once be stated that the reasoning is correct, for within the limits of error of observation they do so. Having established this proposition, we can next compare inter se, the brightness of any or all of the rays of the spectrum by a preliminary comparison with the reflected beam of white light. As in the colour patch apparatus all colours and principal dark lines of the solar spectrum are known by reference to a scale, in making a graphic representation of the results, we first of all plot on paper a scale of equal parts, and at the scale number where a reading is made, the aperture of the sectors in degrees is set up. Thus, suppose with red light the scale number which marked the position of the slit was 59, and the aperture 10°,81 we should set up at that scale number on the paper a height of 10 on any empyric scale. If in the green at scale No. 38 the sectors had to be closed to 7°, we should set up 7 at that number on the scale.

When observations have been made at numerous places in the spectrum, the tops of these ordinates, as they are called, should be joined, and we then get the observed curve of luminosity for the whole spectrum. For convenience’ sake we make the highest point 100, and reduce the other ordinates in proportion. For some purposes it may be advantageous to give the luminosity curve in terms of a scale of wave-lengths. For our purpose, however, it is in general sufficient to use the scale of the instrument.

Fig. 19.

Now, if we test the vision of the various types of colour blind by this plan, we should expect to get luminosities at different parts of the spectrum which would give very different forms to these curves. We cannot hope, for instance, that a red-blind who sees no red in the extreme end of the spectrum would show any luminosity in that region, nor that the green-blind should show as much in the green part of the spectrum as those who possess normal colour vision, since one of the sensations is absent. With monochromatic vision there should be a still further departure from the82 normal curve. That these differences do exist is fully shown in Fig. 19. One of the most striking experiments in colour vision is to place a bright red patch on the screen, and to ask a red-blind to make a match in83 luminosity with the white. The latter will have to be reduced to almost darkness—a darkness, indeed, that makes the match almost seem incredible. You will notice that the places in the spectrum where the red- and green-blind see grey are by no means places of greatest luminosity. We shall find that these luminosity curves are suggestive when making another investigation into the form of the spectrum curves of the colour sensations.

Besides cases of complete blindness due to the absence of one or two sensations on the Young theory, we have other cases, as was said when remarking on the percentage of people who are colour deficient, in which one or even two sensations are only more or less deadened. It has often been said that with the theory provisionally adopted, such cases are difficult to class as red or green deficient. As far as my own observations go, I have never found this difficulty. The luminosity curves of such observers, combined with other indications, give a ready means of classing them. The main difficulty to my mind is to state what is normal colour vision, but, as I have found that the very large majority of eyes give the same luminosity to colours as my own, I have taken my own colour perception as normal. In numerous experiments which Lord84 Rayleigh has made in matching orange by means of a mixture of red and green, he has come across several who have apparently normal vision, as they see colours correctly in every part of the spectrum, and yet some require much less red mixed with the green to make a match with the orange than do others. What is yellow to them is decidedly green to the majority. This has been classed as another kind of normal vision; but the luminosity curves show that it may be equally well due to a deficiency in the green sensation, and which would require more green to make the necessary match. The limits of the visible spectrum to these persons, as far as my examination of their cases goes, are the same as my own.

Again, there are others in which the spectrum seems decidedly somewhat shortened at the red end compared with my own, and the luminosity curves point to them as being strictly colour deficient in the red and nothing else. As they see all colours, they have been classed as another form of normal vision. The deficiency in both these cases is so small that white is their neutral colour, but there is evidence that the hues are slightly changed. I do not wish any one to accept my deductions as being more correct than those who hold differently, but the85 results of examination by the luminosity methods appear to me difficult to reconcile with any other view. There are, however, a large number of cases in which, though complete red- or green-blindness is wanting, there is no doubt that more than slight colour deficiency exists. For instance, in Fig. 23 we have the curve of luminosity of the spectrum as measured by a very acute scientific observer, and it is compared with that of normal colour vision. He certainly is not completely blind to any sensation. An inspection and comparison of the two curves will show that he is defective in the green sensation, although it is present to a large extent. The deficiency is obvious enough. An endeavour to find his neutral point was most interesting. At 39 in the scale he saw a little colour, but at 39·5 all colour had vanished, and between the coloured patch and the white he saw no difference. This similarity he saw till 47·3 in the scale, when he began to see a faint trace of colour. There is a large piece of the spectrum, then, which to him is grey. It must be recollected that all three sensations were excited in this region, but some more than others. Now, experiment has shown that, with normal vision, two per cent. of any colour may be mixed with a pure colour without its being perceived.86 It is not surprising, therefore, that although the red, or the green, or the blue may be present in an intensity above that required to form white, yet the resulting sensation should pass for white. It may be87 remarked that red and white when mixed he never mistook for yellow, and he always recognised yellows and red; yellowish green, however, he called pale yellow.

Fig. 23.

Another example of partial red-blindness is also instructive. Fig. 23 also shows it graphically. There is no doubt as to the nature of the defect. The spectrum is slightly shortened, and the luminosity of this part of the spectrum is less than that of normal vision. There was no difficulty in distinguishing every colour, though the positions of the colours from yellow to green seemed to be shifted; but no neutral point could be traced. Apparently, both this case and the former are about equally colour defective; but in this last the same reasons do not apply for the existence of a neutral point. (For measures see page 214.)


88

CHAPTER VII.

We are now in a position to carry the investigations as to luminosity a little further. When we look at small patches of light, we view the colour through the yellow spot in the eye. If, when we have matched the luminosity in the ordinary manner, we turn our eyes some 10° away from the patches, we shall find that except at one place in the green the equality in brightness no longer exists. By a little practice we can make matches of luminosity when the eyes are thus diverted. This will give us a different curve of luminosity, as the yellow spot absorption is absent, and the difference in the heights of the ordinates between the two curves will give us that absorption. Fig. 20 shows this very well; and it will be noticed that the eye is appreciably not so sensitive to the red and yellow at 10° from the axis as it is on its central area. If we measure the areas of these two curves we get the relative values of the light energy which is89 active on the two parts of the eye, and these we found to be as 167 to 156. The heights at which to put the90 maxima of the two curves were found from various considerations, and the correctness of the deductions was verified by directly comparing the intensities of two patches of white light some 10° apart, which, when looked at direct, were of equal intensities. When one was compared with the other, the eye receiving one image centrally and the other outside the yellow spot, the difference in values was closely proportional to those of the above areas. The part of this last curve showing a deficiency in red sensation is very similar to that obtained from a person who is partially colour blind. The absorption by the yellow spot derived from these measures is graphically shown in the next figure (Fig. 21).

Fig. 20.
Fig. 21.

The question of the visual sensation at the “fovea centralis” (if it be admitted that this is coincident with the visual axis of the eye, as is usually accepted) may be very easily studied. When the luminosity of the spectrum is examined at five or six feet distance, by throwing the two patches on the whitened face of a small square of half-inch side, we get a result differing from both of the above. The fovea appears to be slightly more sensitive to red than the macula lutea, and is generally less sensitive to the green rays (see Fig. 20). If a star, or a distant light, be observed with91 the part of the retina, on which the axis of the eye falls, as is the case in ordinary vision, and then be observed92 with the eye slightly directed away, the difference in the colours of the light is unmistakable. (The tables giving the measured value of these curves will be found in the appendix, page 211.)

Fig. 22.

Can we in any way find from these methods the colour sensation curves? I think we can. Suppose we have a second instrument exactly like the first placed side by side with it, we can then throw two patches of colour on the two adjacent white surfaces, and we can mix with either, or both of them, as much white light as we choose. From the second instrument let us throw all the spectrum colours in succession on to the one surface, and on to the other the three primary colours mixed in such proportions as to match them accurately. This plan is, I venture to think, a better way of obtaining the value of colours in terms of standard colours than that adopted by Maxwell. This method gives the values directly, and not by calculation from matches with white. Let us place one slit near each of the extreme ends of the spectrum; that in the red near the red lithium line, and another a little beyond G in the violet of the spectrum, whilst the third slit should be in the exact position in the green, where the green-blind sees grey. Now it might be a matter of dispute93 as to whether one was entitled to make this last one of the positions for the slits, for we use it entirely on the assumption that two of the colour sensations which we suppose we possess are identical with those of the green-blind. This might be, or might not be, the case; but I think it can be shown very easily that the assumption we are making is more than probably exact. Having the slits in these positions, we may endeavour to match the spectrum orange. We mix the red and the green lights together, and find that the best mixture is always paler than the orange, but by adding a small quantity of white to the orange we at once form a match. In the same way if we have a greenish-blue to match, we shall find that we can only make the match when we add a little white to the simple colour. Now let us shift the position of the slit in the green just a little—a very little—towards the blue, and again try to match orange. Do what we will we cannot find apertures to the slits which will give us the colour, though it be diluted with white. It will be too blue or too red, but never exactly orange. This tells us that there is too much blue in the green we are using. Next, shift the slit a little towards the red below our fixed position, and endeavour to match the blue. We shall find that this, too, becomes impracticable.94 The blue is either too green or too violet, telling us that our mixture contains too much green. As the neutral point of the colour blind is the only position for the green slit which enables us to make a good match to both the orange and the blue, it follows that this must be the point where these two colour sensations are so arranged as to be in the proportions required to form white when green is added; that is, that there is neither an excess of red nor an excess of violet. To come back to our measures of colour. We can make up every spectrum colour with these three colours, and finally divide the luminosity curve into the colour luminosity. In all these matches the violet luminosity is very small indeed compared with the red or green. A match with white is now made by a mixture of all these colours, and you will see, from the images of the slits on the screen, that the luminosity of the violet is almost a negligible quantity compared with the others. We may, therefore, as a first approximation, divide up the luminosity curve into two parts, one being the luminosity of the green in the different colours and the other of the red. The green, however, is made up of red, of violet, and of an excess of green sensation, which in this case comes practically to a mixture of white with the green sensation. How95 can we tell how much is green and how much is white? Suppose I, as a normal-eyed person, compare the luminosity of the colour coming through the red slit with that coming through the green slit, and then get the green-blind to do the same, it is evident that any excess in the luminosity as measured by myself over that measured by the green-blind must be due to the green sensation, and we can also see how much red and violet make up his white. We shall not be far wrong, then, in apportioning the constituents of the white thus found between the green and the red; the violet being, for the time being, negligible. We must subtract the red sensation from the green colour curve and add it to the red colour curve: the two curves will then be very closely the curves of the red and green sensations. By causing the green-blind to make mixtures of red and violet for all the colours of their spectrum, we can arrive at what must be finally taken away or given to these curves, though such addition or subtraction of violet will be small when the luminosities are considered. The accompanying figure (Fig. 22) gives an idea of the96 shape and general features of these curves. It may be remarked that we can check the general accuracy of the measures of the colour mixtures by calculating or measuring the areas of the two colour curves, the red and the green. If accurate, they should bear the same ratio that the luminosities of the two colours bear to each other (when mixed with a little violet, which is practically negligible) to form white light. So far, then, we can utilize the luminosity methods to calculate and to trace the sensation curves for the normal eye. It will not escape your notice that the maximum heights of these two component curves are nowhere near the parts of the spectrum where the colour is the purest. Another check to these curves may also be made by taking the difference in the ordinates of the luminosity curves of the colour blind and the normal eyed. Too much stress must not, however, for the moment, be laid on this, as this method depends on the absolute correctness of the scale of the ordinates in the curves. It must be recollected that to the former white light is deprived of at least one constituent sensation which is perceived by normal eyes. This, in all probability, renders the white less luminous to them than those possessing normal vision, so that the comparisons of luminosities are referred to different standards.

97

It may seem a very simple matter to ascertain the correct scale, but it is not, except by the extinction method, which will be described later. At one time General Festing and myself tried to obtain a comparison by finding the limiting illumination at which a book could be read. We got results, but for the purpose in question the values are not conclusive. What we really were measuring was the acuteness of vision in different coloured lights. As a good deal depends upon the optical perfection of the eyes under examination, besides the illumination, we must be on our guard, even if there were nothing else against the method, against taking any such measures as being conclusive.


98

CHAPTER VIII.

Before quitting the measurement of luminosity, it may be as well to see whether the curves described are the same whatever the brilliancy of the spectrum may be. We can easily experiment with a very reduced brightness. Upon the screen we have an ordinarily bright spectrum. As the slit, through which the white light forming the spectrum comes, is narrowed, there is an evident change in the relative brightness of the different parts, though the energy of every ray must be proportionally reduced. The red is much more enfeebled than the green, and in brightness the green part of the spectrum looks much more intense than the yellow, which is ordinarily the brightest part. This we have assured ourselves of not only by casual observation, but also by direct measurement. Perhaps I can make this even more decisive to you. Two slits are now in the ordinarily bright spectrum, one in the red, and the other in a green which is99 near the E line of the solar spectrum. Instead of using one lens to form a single colour patch of mixed light, two parts of a lens, appropriately cut and of the same focal length as the large combining lens, are placed in front of the slits, one bit of lens before each. This artifice enables us to throw the patch of red on one white surface, and the patch of green light on another adjacent to it. By opening or closing one or other of the slits the brightness of the two patches of light are so arranged that there is no manner of doubt but that the red is the brighter of the two. The absolute energies of the rays forming each of the patches are proportionally reduced by closing the slit of the collimator, as before. At one stage both patches appear of about the same intensity. This might be taken for an error in judgment, but to make the change that takes place perfectly plain to you, the rotating sectors are introduced in front of the two slits, and the rays now pass through them. The apertures of the sectors are gradually closed, and we now come to such a reduction that the red is absolutely invisible; but the green still shines out. It is losing its colour somewhat, and appears of a bluish tint. The reason of this change of hue in the latter we shall shortly see. The sectors are withdrawn100 and the red re-appears, and is as bright as the green. The slit of the collimator is next opened, and there is no doubt that the red is much brighter than the green, as it was purposely made at the beginning of the experiment. The same class of experiment might have been repeated with the green and violet or the red and violet, and the same kind of results would have been obtained. The violet would have been the last to disappear when the green was so reduced in luminosity that it appeared in the ordinary brilliant spectrum to be equal to the violet ray selected. When the green was of the luminosity given by a slit equal in width to that of the violet, the violet would have disappeared first, owing to its feeble brightness to begin with. Now, if we measure a feebly illuminated spectrum we must adopt some special means to exclude all light, except that of the comparison light and the ray to be measured. This we can do by the box which is shown in the next diagram (Fig. 24).

Fig. 24.

At one end of a box, shown in plan, is an eye-piece, E. The other end has at its centre a white square of paper of 1½-inch scale. The monochromatic beam a, coming from the spectrum through the slit S and the reference beam b of white light,101 are reflected from glass mirrors M₁, M₂ to apertures in opposite sides of the box, and from close to these apertures by the right-angled prisms P₁ P₂, so as to fall on and cover S. Rods R₁, R₂ are inserted in the102 box in the paths of the beams, so that the opposite halos of S are illuminated. Diaphragms inside the box cut off any stray light, and rotating sectors placed at A and B regulate the intensity of the beams as required. The sector A is rotated with a previously determined-on aperture; the white light coming through B is altered till the luminosity of the two on the screen, as seen through E, are the same. Every part of the spectrum can be measured in this way; the result is shown in the diagram. Fig. 25 (the measures will be found at page 215 in the appendix). In this case the orange light at D where it fell on the screen was equal to 1/132 of an amyl-acetate light, which, in its turn, is closely ·8 of a standard candle. In the same figure the luminosity curve of the ordinary bright spectrum is given for reference, and it can be seen how the point of maximum luminosity is shifted into the green, lying almost over the E line of the solar spectrum. The maximum, of course, has been made 100 as before, for had it been drawn to the same scale as the other, the form of the curve would not have been demonstrated. There is a remarkable resemblance between it and the curve of luminosity of the monochromatic vision, and such a resemblance can scarcely be103 fortuitous. As a matter of fact, in this we seem to have come to the final curve for low luminosities,104 and is almost the same as that observed when the spectrum is reduced to such an extent that it is colourless throughout, a condition that it can assume, as we shall see very shortly. When the spectrum is rather more luminous, it gives a curve of luminosity which is similar to that of the ordinary spectrum when measured by a red-blind person. Here, then, we have an indication that a person with normal vision passes through a stage of red-blindness, as the intensity is diminished before he arrives at absolutely monochromatic vision.

Fig. 25.

This investigation is of practical as well as theoretical interest, as General Festing and myself quickly discovered when we first made it. The curious colour of a moonlight landscape is entirely accounted for by it. White light becomes greenish-blue as it diminishes in intensity, and the reds and yellows, being reduced or absent, are not reflected by surrounding objects. Hence, moonlight is cold, whilst the sunlight is warm owing to their presence.

When measuring these low luminosities, the various colours will in a great measure disappear. Part of the spectrum will be of that peculiar grey which was shown you in the experiment with the incandescent light (p. 34). By further experiment it105 is possible to arrive at an approximate determination of the point where all colour vanishes from the different parts of the spectrum. We use the same apparatus (Fig. 24) as before, the only difference being that each of the sectors is movable during rotation. The apertures of those through which the colour passes are reduced till all colour on the screen just disappears, the point being arrived at by a comparison with the white, which is itself also reduced. The apertures of the first sector alone need be noted, and from these readings the diagram (Fig. 26) is made (for measures, see page 216).

Fig. 26.

This extinction of colour is one which often occurs, but is seldom noticed. The figure tells us that the orange is about the last colour of the spectrum left, some of the others still appearing as greys. The next to retain its colour is the green, and the most rapid to lose them are the red and violet. It must not be supposed that the colours remain of the same hue up to the time that they vanish. Pure spectrum red (red sensation) remains the same up to the last, but the scarlet becomes orange, and the orange yellower, and the green bluer. This is what would be predicted from the Young theory if the order of extinction of sensation be red, green, violet. This107 we shall see is the case. At nightfall in the summer the order of disappearance of colour may often be seen; orange flowers may be plainly visible, yet a red geranium may appear black as night; the green grass will be grey when the colour of the yellow flowers may yet be just visible. An early morning start in the autumn before daybreak will give an ample opportunity of satisfying oneself as to the order in which colours gradually re-appear as daybreak approaches. Red flowers will be at the outset black, whilst other colours will be visible as grey. As more light comes from the sky the pale yellow and blue flowers will next be distinguished, though the grass may still be a nondescript grey. Then, as the light still increases, every colour will burst out, if not in their full brilliance, yet into their own undoubted hue.


108

CHAPTER IX.

Not only, however, may we lose a sense of colour, but we may also lose all sense of light by reducing the energy of the different rays. We have seen that colour goes unequally from the different parts of the spectrum. We may therefore prognosticate that the light itself may disappear more rapidly from some parts than from others. You will scarcely, however, I think, be prepared for the enormous difference which exists in the stages of disappearance of the grey of the reduced red and of that of the reduced green.

Fig. 27.

But how are we to measure this extinction of light at the different parts of the spectrum? This is a problem which I have attacked during the last few years by a variety of methods; but as is the case with almost every scientific problem, when the mode of attack is reduced to its simplest form, it yields the more readily to solution. If we have a box, like that figured in Fig. 27, and combine it with our109 colour patch apparatus, the problem is solved. B B is a closed box 3 feet long and about 1 foot high and wide, having two similar apertures 1½ inch in diameter in the positions shown. The aperture at the side is covered on the inside by a piece of glass a, ground on both sides, and a tube T is inserted, in which diaphragms, D, of various apertures can be inserted at pleasure. The most convenient form of diaphragm is that supplied with photographic lenses—an iris diaphragm. E is a tube fitted at the end of the box through which the screen S is viewed. S is black except in the centre, where a white disc is fastened to it. A mirror, M, placed as shown, reflects the light scattered by the ground glass on to the screen S. The rotating sectors are placed where shown, and are in such a position that they can be readily adjusted by the observer. The patch of any desired colour of the110 spectrum is thrown on a, and an appropriate size of diaphragm used, so that when the sectors are not less than 5° to 10° open, the light totally disappears. We can now make observations throughout the whole spectrum, and knowing the value of the different apertures of the diaphragm and the angular opening of the rotating sectors, we can at once find the amount of reduction of the particular part of the spectrum that is being required in order to just extinguish all traces of light from the white disc at the end of the box. From these measures we can readily construct a curve or curves which will graphically show the reduction given to the different parts of the spectrum. Fig. 28 gives the curve of extinction for ordinary normal colour vision. The spectrum was of such a brilliance that the intensity of the square patch of light formed on a of the orange light (D) was exactly that of an amyl-acetate lamp, placed at one foot distance from the receiving screen. Knowing this, the actual luminosity of all the other rays of the spectrum can be derived from the curve of luminosity (see Fig. 20). Extinguishing the various parts of the spectrum by this plan, it is found that the red rays cease to stimulate the retina sufficiently to give any111 appearance of light long before the green rays are extinguished. It is only the rays in the extreme violet of the spectrum, and which consequently possess very feeble luminosity, that make any approach112 towards requiring the same amount of reduction as the red rays.

Fig. 28.

There is the fact to remember in making these measures in the extreme red and the extreme violet, that the luminosities of the colours are so small that the illumination of the prism itself, by the white light falling on it, has to be taken into account, since it forms an appreciable portion of the patch of feeble colour. By placing a proper shade of blue or red glass in the front of the collimator slit this white light disappears or becomes negligible, and when the absorption of the coloured glass is known from measurement, we can get a very accurate measure of the extinction of these parts. Some people may propound the idea that the rotating sectors may in such kind of measurements give a false result. Now such a criticism is quite fair, and it is absolutely necessary that it should be answered. Well, to test the accuracy or the reverse of the assumption that such measures are correct, the following small piece of simple apparatus was devised. A and B (Fig. 29) are two mirrors placed at angles of 45° to the angle of incidence of the beam. The path the beam takes can be readily ascertained from the figure. This piece of apparatus was placed in position in front of the spectrum, and the reflected113 beams used to form the patches of colour. For convenience only a small pencil of light was allowed to issue from the prism, a diaphragm of some ½-inch in diameter being placed in front of it. This allows a spot of any desired colour to fall on the screen, the ground glass being removed. The slit through which the spectrum colours pass is moved along the spectrum, and a position is arrived at where the last glimmer of light disappears.

Fig. 29.

The mirrors A and B may both be of plain glass blackened with smoke on one side, or one may be plain glass and one silvered, or they both may be silvered. This, with the power possessed of altering the aperture of the slit of collimator, puts us in possession of ample means of making our measures. We may also use the ground-glass arrangement and use different diaphragms, which puts a further power of variation in our hands. I may at once state that the resulting measurements fell on the curves, obtained by measurements made with the rotating sectors, a sufficient proof that the sectors may be used with confidence. There is still another method which avoids a resort114 to the sectors. A tapering wedge of black glass can be moved in front of the colour slit, and a different thickness of glass will be required to cause the extinction of each colour. Recently I have modified the extinction box, more particularly for the purpose of using it where the spectrum is to be formed of a feeble light, such as that of an incandescent lamp or a candle. If a really black wedge could be obtained, this would seem to be the best method, but no glass is really black. We have, therefore, to make a preliminary study of the wedge to ascertain accurately the absorption co-efficients for the different rays, a piece of work which requires a good deal of patience, but which, when done, is always at command.

In Fig. 28 two branches of the curves are given at the blue end of the spectrum; one is shown as the extinction for the centre of the eye, and the other of the whole eye. Of course the former observations were made by looking direct at the spot. This may appear a very easy matter, but it is not really so simple as it sounds. It is curious how little control there is over the absolute direction of the eyes when the light has almost disappeared. The axes of the eyes are often directed to quite a different point. When115 the extinction for the whole eye is made, the readings are really much easier, as then the eye roams where it likes, and a final disappearance is noted. When the eye has once been invested with a roving commission, it is hard to control it. In making these observations it was therefore advisable to have data for the first branch of the curve, before commencing to observe for the later. The main cause of difference between the two branches of the curve is due to the absorption by the yellow spot.

It might be thought that with the curves (Fig. 28) before us, we have learnt all we can regarding the extinction of light, but is it so? Surely we ought to know something as to the reduction necessary for extinction of the different parts of the spectrum when they are all of equal luminosities and of ordinary brightness.

We arrive at this by simple calculation. Supposing we have two luminosities, one double the other, it does not require much thought to find out that you have to reduce the greater luminosity twice as much as the other in order for it to be just extinguished. In other words, if we multiply the extinction by the luminosity, we get what we want. Now, in the curves before us, we have taken the luminosity of the yellow light near116 D as one amyl-acetate lamp, and that has a height in the curve showing the spectrum luminosity very closely approaching 100. We may, therefore, multiply the extinctions of a ray by the value of its ordinate in the luminosity curve and divide the result by 100, and this will give us the extinction of each colour, supposing it had the luminosity of an amyl-acetate lamp. A portion of the curve so calculated is shown in the same diagram (Fig. 28) as a dotted line. It appears at the violet end as an approximately horizontal line, and then starts rapidly upwards, and would, if carried on to the same scale, reach far out of the diagram; but at the extreme red it would be found to bend and again become horizontal. I would have you notice that the same is true not only for the extinction observed with the centre of the eye through the yellow spot, but also for the whole eye. Such straight, horizontal parts of the curve must mean something.

Fig. 30.

In the diagram (Fig. 16) of colour sensations we see that in each of these two regions there is but one sensation excited, viz. the violet and the red. Now, if these sensation curves mean anything, the reduction necessary to produce the extinction of the same sensation when equally stimulated should prove to be the118 same, for there is no reason to the contrary, but exactly the reverse. Primâ facie, then, taking the Young theory as correct, we may suppose that these horizontal parts are due to the extinction of one sensation. Let us treat it as such, and go back to the original extinction curve shown in the continuous lines. The parts of the curve which lie over the fairly horizontal dotted line, at all events, should be the extinction curve of the same sensation, but more or less stimulated or excited. As before explained, if we have double the stimulation at one part of the spectrum to that we have at another, the reduction of the greater luminosity to give extinction will be double that of the lesser. If, then, we take the reciprocals of the extinction, it ought to give us a curve which is of the form of some colour sensation; and when we arrive at the maximum, we may for convenience make that ordinate 100, and reduce the other ordinates proportionally. This has been done in Fig. 30 in the curves C and D. For the sake of a name my colleague and myself have named such curves “persistency curves.” Perhaps some other name might be more fitting; but still a poor name is better than none at all.

When the persistency curve was scrutinized to see119 what might be taken as its full signification, I must confess that the result astonished us somewhat, though we ought not to have been surprised. The persistency curve C, when applied (in a Euclidean sense) to the curve of luminosity recorded for the men who had monochromatic vision, almost exactly coincided with it. In other words, by far the largest part of the extinction was due to the extinction of the sensation which in the monochromatic vision was alone excited. If this be not the case, there is something in colour vision which no theory which I am acquainted with can account for. Then, again, the persistency curve agrees with the curve of luminosity when the intensity of the spectrum is very feeble, which is another coincidence of a remarkable character which some theory should explain. [Fig. 30 gives, besides the persistency curves, the luminosity curves of the normal eye, of monochromatic vision, and of the violet-blind; and an exaggerated curve of the difference between the normal luminosity curve and that of the violet-blind, and others which I think will be found useful for general reference.]

What sensation is it that is last extinguished, and which is possessed by a certain class of colour vision?120 In the Young theory it can only be the violet sensation. It is certainly not the green, and much less the red. It does not correspond, however, very well with the violet sensation shown in Fig. 16, but more with one which should be in the blue.

In making the extinctions of light, it is quite necessary that certain precautions should be taken to avoid error. All my audience know that when going from bright daylight into a cellar, in which only a glimmer of light is admitted, but little can be seen at first, but that, as the eye “gets accustomed” to the darkness, the surroundings will begin to be seen, and after several minutes what before was blackness comes to be invested with form and detail. So it is with the extinction of light in the apparatus described. Observations carried on before the full sensibility of the eye is attained are of no value. A recorded set of observations will show this. A light of a certain character was thrown on the extinction box, to be extinguished, and the observer entered the darkened room from the full glare of daylight. The eye was placed at the eye end and kept there, and the extinctions were made one after the other till they became very fairly constant. The following is the result:—

121

Times of Observation. Readings.
At the commencement     1·0
After              38 sec.     3·2
After              53 sec.     4·9
After   1 min. 11 sec.     6·9
After   1 min. 44 sec.   10·5
After   2 min. 43 sec.   17·0
After   3 min. 44 sec.   27·5
After   4 min. 52 sec.   43·0
After   5 min. 59 sec.   63·0
After   6 min. 41 sec.   78·0
After   7 min. 28 sec.   89·0
After   8 min. 32 sec.   96·0
After 10 min. 46 sec. 103·0
After 12 min. 103·0

(For convenience the first reading is unity; the other numbers are the inverse of the extinction value.)

The eye apparently, under the conditions in which these observations were made, was at least 100 times more sensitive to very faint light after twelve minutes than it was at the beginning, and that then concordant readings could be made. It will now be quite understood that before any serious measures can be made this interval must elapse, and also that the light, finding its way to the end of the box to illuminate the spot, should never be strong, otherwise the eye might lose its sensitiveness.


122

CHAPTER X.

Before considering the subject of the extinction of light by other types of colour vision, attention must be called to what has already been brought before you. The various colours of the spectrum have to be reduced to the following amounts before they suffer extinction, the orange light at D being of the value of one candle. (See appendix, page 217, for complete tables.)

Reduction in
Millionths.
Remarks.
B 10,000    or 1/100            approximately pure red sensation
C 1,100    or 1/909          rather more scarlet
D 50    or 1/20000 orange light
E     6·5 or 1/154000 a green chosen by Maxwell as a standard colour
F 15·0 or 1/67000 beginning of the blue
Blue Lithium 85·0 or 1/11700 a good sample of blue
G 300·0 or 1/3300     approximately pure sensation of violet.

If we make these same colours all of the luminosity of one amyl-acetate lamp (·8 of a candle), we find that the numbers are as follows:—

123

   Reduction in
Millionths.
B 300    
C 225    
D   48    
E     3·3
F       ·9
Blue Lithium     1·1
G     1·1

These numbers are remarkable, and we may enforce what they mean in this way. The energy of radiation, and of light also when of ordinary luminosity, varies inversely as the square of the distance from an incandescent body when of small dimensions. But from the above it seems that a white screen receiving the rays from an amyl-acetate lamp in an otherwise perfectly dark place, and having a colour which stimulates the red sensation alone, would be invisible at 58 feet distance, for there would not be enough energy transmitted to stimulate the red perceiving apparatus sufficiently to give the sensation of light. If it were an orange light, such as sodium, of the same luminosity, we should have to move it from the screen 142 feet before the same result was attained. With the green light at E, the distance would be 550 feet, and with the violet the distance would be increased to 1000 feet. The reduction in intensity of white light, which, when of ordinary brightness, is warm, would make it colder, for the red would disappear, and finally the residue of light, just before extinction, would become a cold grey,124 due to the absence of all colour. The changes in hue that would occur are variable, the variation being due to the loss of colour of the different rays for different amounts of reduction, and then their final extinction. We can place two patches of white light on the screen, and gradually reduce one in intensity, keeping the other of its original value. No one would expect that the two would be dissimilar in hue, as they appear to be when the former is moderately near the extinction value. If we wish to see this perfectly, we should use an extinction box and view it away from the surroundings, which must be more or less slightly illuminated.

It has already been stated that the persistency curve for persons who have normal colour vision is closely the same as that recorded for those who are of the monochromatic type. As this is so, we must expect to find that the persistency curve of these last is the same as their luminosity curve. We put this to the test of experiment and found that our reasoning was correct, for the persistency curve could be almost exactly fitted to it. (See table, pages 217 and 222.) The slight difference between them can be credited to the fact that the whole eye may have been brought into use during the extinction observations, the centre of the eye not being exclusively used. The Figure 31125 shows both the extinction and the persistency curves, and also the curve of luminosity for the normal eye.

Fig. 31.

126

The former were derived from a case P. sent for examination. P. and Q. are brothers, each of whom possesses but one colour sensation, and examination showed that their vision was identical. Mr. Nettleship has kindly given me the following particulars regarding them:—“Their acutes of vision (form vision) in ordinary daylight is only one-tenth of the normal. A younger sister and brother are idiotic and almost totally blind, and in one of these the optic nerves show clear evidence of disease. Hence, the colour blindness of P. and Q. must almost without doubt be considered as the result of disease, perhaps ante-natal, involving some portion of the visual apparatus.” A lack of acuteness of vision would be expected from the small amount of light they perceive compared with normal vision. The fact that two of a family, not twins, possess exactly the same colour sense, and that their extinction curves are entirely different to those suffering from post-natal disease, but similar to those of normal vision, point to their colour blindness as falling in the same general category as that of the congenital type. To this I shall refer again.

Fig. 32.

We may reason still further. With the red- and green-blind the violet sensation is still present, and we127 may therefore expect that their extinction curves, and consequently their persistency curves, should be alike, and should also agree with that made from your lecturer’s observations. A study of Figures 32 and 33 will tell you that such is practically the case. The former shows the luminosity, the persistency, and the extinction curves of a completely red-blind subject, and the latter the same curves for a green-blind subject (see pages 223 and 224). Both were excellent observers, and their examination was easy, owing to the acquaintance with scientific methods. The accuracy of their results may be taken as unquestionable. Each of them may be taken as a representative of their own particular type of colour blindness. There is an agreement between them at the violet end, but a deviation at the red end of the spectrum. The general form of the curves indicates that the same sensation is extinguished last in all. Now, have we any other criterion to offer? We have. In the first instance, we have the violet-blind person to compare with the others, and also another observer who had monochromatic vision, but whose sensation was different to that of the two monochromatic cases we have so far brought to your notice. We have already stated the peculiarities in colour nomenclature of the violet-blind case. His curve of luminosity for the spectrum128 was taken (page 227), and when compared with the curve of normal luminosity, it became evident that in129 the red and up to the orange his measures were those which a normal eye would make; but that the luminosity fell off in the green, and finally disappeared to an immeasurable quantity in the violet (see Fig. 30, curves M and F). If his measures of spectrum luminosity are deducted from those of the normal eye, and the ordinates be increased proportionately to make the maximum difference 100, the figure so produced, when compared with the luminosity curve obtained from the monochromatic observers, was found to be the same, and consequently with the persistency curves above referred to. Endeavours were made to gain a good extinction curve, but the results were not as successful as could be desired; but it was ascertained that, without doubt, his most persistent sensation was not more than 1/180 as lasting as that of the normal eye, or to put it in another way, his green at E was only extinguished when the energy falling on his eye was 180 times greater than that at which it vanished with the normal eye. This plainly teaches us that the missing sensation was that which, when present, is ordinarily the most persistent.

The next is a case of monochromatic vision, which differs from those previously brought before you, and I cannot do better than describe it in the words which130 General Festing and myself employed in our paper in the “Philosophical Transactions.”

Fig. 33.

131

The patient (B. C.) had been examined by Mr. Nettleship, who kindly secured his attendance at South Kensington for the purpose of being examined by the spectrum and other tests. [Mr. Nettleship states that this case is without doubt a genuine case of congenital colour blindness, without any trace whatever of disease.] B. C. is a youth of 19, who has served as an apprentice at sea. His form vision is perfect, and he is not night blind. He can see well at all times, though he states that on a cloudy day his vision seemed to be slightly more acute than in sunshine. He was first requested to make matches with the Holmgren wools in the usual manner, with the result that he was found to possess monochromatic vision. He matched reds, greens, blues, dark yellows, browns, greys, and purples together; and it was a matter of chance if he selected any proper match for any of the test colours. Finally, when pressed, he admitted that the whole of the heap of wools were “blue” to him, any one only differing from another in brightness. The brighter colours he called “dirty” or “pale” blue, terms which eventually proved to be synonymous. We then examined him with patches of monochromatic spectrum colours by means of the colour patch apparatus. He designated every colour as “blue,” except a bright yellow,132 which he called white, but when the luminosity of this colour was reduced he pronounced it a good blue. So with white, as the illumination was decreased, he pronounced it to pass first into dirty blue, and then into a full blue.

Colour discs were then brought into requisition, and it was hard at first to know how to make the necessary alterations, owing to the terms he employed to express the difference which existed between the inner disc and the outer grey ring. By noting that a pale “blue” passed into a pure blue when the amount of white in the outer ring was diminished, and that the inner disc was described as “pale” or “dirty” when the outer ring was described as “a very full blue,” we were enabled to make him match accurately a red, a green, and a blue disc separately with mixtures of black and white.

The following are the equations:—

360 red     = 315 black + 45 white.
360 green = 258 black + 102 white.
360 blue   = 305 black + 55 white.

With these proportions he emphatically stated that all were good blues, and that the inner disc and outer ring were identical in brightness and in colour.

It may be remarked that this is a case of congenital colour blindness, and that there is reason to believe that some of his ancestors were colour blind.

133

Before using the discs an attempt was made to ascertain the luminosity of the spectrum as it appeared to him. His readings, however, were so erratic that nothing could be made out from these first observations, except to fix the place of maximum luminosity, the terms “pale” and “dirty” puzzling us as to their real meanings. After the experience with the discs we had a clue as to what he wished to express by pale or dirty blue, which only meant that the colour or white was too bright, and on making a second attempt he matched the luminosities of the two shadows as easily as did P. and Q., the other cases of monochromatic vision. The method adopted was to diminish the white light illuminating one shadow to the point at which he pronounced it a good blue, when a slight alteration in the intensity was always sufficient to secure to his eye equality of luminosity between it and the coloured shadow without his perceiving any alteration in the saturation.

Fig. 34.

B. C.’s Luminosity and Extinction Curves.

The curve of luminosity, Fig. 34, is a very remarkable one, being different in character to that of P. and Q., the maximum being well on the D side of E. A great falling off in the luminosity when compared with that measured by the normal eye will be noticed both in the blue and in the red. (For measures see page 225.) The evidence134 was therefore presumptive that B. C.’s colour sensation was neither red nor blue, but probably a green.

135

The next test was made to throw light on this point. He made observations of the extinction of the different parts of the spectrum. His observations were very fair, except on the violet side of F, where they became slightly erratic, but by requesting him to use all parts of his retina to obtain the last glimpse of light, a very concordant curve resulted, as shown in Fig. 34. Some of his observations at this part were evidently made with the centre of the retina, for they gave readings which, when the “persistency” curve was calculated, and these observations treated as part of the extinction, agreed with the luminosity curve. We may, therefore, conclude that B. C. has a region in the retina in which there is an absorbing medium corresponding to the yellow spot of the normal eyed. This is diagrammatically shown in Fig. 34 by the difference in height of ordinates in the persistency (dotted) and the luminosity curves. On the red side of the maximum the two curves are practically identical, except from Scale number 54. At this point it is probable that the white light which illuminated the prism vitiated the readings to some degree. At the violet end something similar, doubtless, occurs, but it is masked by the difference that exists in the extinction by the central part of the retina and that of the whole eye.

136

It must, however, be remarked that the amount of reduction of the intensity of a ray to produce extinction is very different for B. C. and for the normal eyed, or for the red- and green-blind or for P. and Q. B. C. can bear nearly 200 times less reduction for the rays near E. We have already pointed out that the same is practically the case with M., whom we presume to be violet-blind. We may therefore deduce the fact that the monochromatic vision in this case is of a totally different type to that of P. and Q., and that the last sensation to be lost is the same as that of M. If any violet sensation were present in either, the fact would be made evident by the order of the extinction. The sensation of B. C. is thus apparently the green sensation, though that this particular sensation is exactly the same as that absent in the green-blind is not certain.

The observations made by the different types of the colour blind seem to me to throw great light on the theory of colour vision. They show that when the violet sensation is present, according to the Young theory, the extinction shows its presence; and that where this sensation is absent, the reduction of light necessary to produce extinction is greatly less, and may with great certainty be attributed to a different sensation being the final one to disappear.


137

CHAPTER XI.

I have so far spoken only of normal, or physiological, colour blindness; a peculiarity, or defect, present at birth, and, as far as is at present known, irremediable, but not associated with any defect of the visual functions, or with any disease or any optical peculiarities. What the nature and seat of this defect may be—whether in the eye or in the sensorium—is at present unknown, although some of the characteristics of the deficiency in colour sensation, I believe, seem to indicate the existence of a special part of the brain endowed with the functions for perceiving colour.

But cases are well known to medical men in which colour vision, normal to start with, fails in greater or less degree in connection with disease. This part of the subject is large and very complex, and requires for its full elucidation an acquaintance with the diseases and disorders of the eye. Many of the phenomena accompanying acquired colour blindness, however, are138 of great interest to the physicist in his study of colour vision, more particularly in regard to the test of the truth of any particular theory. Through the kindness of several medical men, and Mr. Nettleship in particular, I have had the opportunity of examining by the colour apparatus several types of colour blindness due to disease. One feature, common, I understand, to all, or nearly all, cases, is the presence of some disease of the optic nerve. Defective sight—from loss of transparency of the cornea, the crystalline lens, or other transparent parts of the eye—does not interfere with the perception of colour; nor is true colour blindness, as I am informed, well marked, if present at all, in disease limited to the choroid and retina (see Fig. 1). Even in cases of the disease of the optic nerve, medical authorities tell us that great differences exist in the amount of colour defect, and that although the colour defect always goes along with some other serious visual loss, either of form, light, or field, the relation between these several factors of the visual defect is by no means always the same, so far as can be judged by the tests commonly used by ophthalmic surgeons. They tell us that in some cases of disease of the optic nerve, colour vision when tested by the wool test, which will be described shortly, may be almost perfect, whilst the139 capacity for reading test letters of the alphabet may be extremely bad, and vice versâ. It seems that in some cases these discrepancies cannot be accounted for; but in others the facts can be explained by the limitation of the disease to certain fibres of the optic nerve. Thus, if those fibres which supply the yellow spot region of the retina are alone involved, direct, or central, vision will be much damaged both for form and colour, whilst a little further from the centre of the field, the visual functions in such a case are often quite normal. From what has been said in the opening chapters, this will be understood to be that the colour vision is perfect, but the definition of form more or less imperfect. We are told that cases of this type have long been known and are comparatively common, and often favourable as regards recovery; that the mischief may affect one optic nerve, or both; that when both are diseased the malady is usually due to the action of some toxic substance, and that of all substances known to have this particular effect on the optic nerves tobacco is the most important. I dwell a little on this variety—damage to form and colour sense at the centre of the visual field of each eye from limited, and usually curable, disease of the optic nerve—on account of its interest to myself in the investigations I have made,140 and also on account of the degree of practical importance which it assumes in connection with the proper reading of signals and coloured lights. These cases of “tobacco amblyopia,” as it is pathologically called, are, of course, always found in men; and it may occasionally happen that such a man, if an engine driver, signalman, or a look-out man on board ship, may still see form sufficiently well to see his signals, but may mistake their true colours. From evidence given before the Committee of the Royal Society on Colour Vision, it appears that the disease causing this type of colour blindness is usually produced by the over-use of tobacco, aided by mental depression and a low state of health. As we have no sumptuary laws, cases of tobacco blindness must frequently occur, and it should be the care of all who have the management of railways or shipping to take measures for preventing persons suffering from this disease from occupying posts which require perfect colour vision in order to prevent the possibility of loss of life.

Congenital colour blindness can at once be discovered, and its possessor be excluded from any post in which normal colour perception is necessary, but with this type a single examination is no safeguard, as it may be developed at any period of a man’s career. The141 disease is, I believe, a progressive one, and at first is most generally unrecognised, the deficiencies of vision being usually slight at its commencement. It is very often brought to the notice of the sufferer by finding he is unable to read. The words at first seem only slightly indistinct, but later become undecipherable, and as time goes on he is unable to even see the letters. He or his friends then usually think it time to consult the specialist. In tobacco amblyopia the area of insensibility is central, and it may subtend a very small angle or one which covers a considerable portion of the field. I am not aware that it ever extends over it all, but it very generally covers the yellow spot. Now as the eye naturally receives the image on the centre of the retina, it follows that, as the ability to distinguish some colours is absent in that particular region, the patient is practically colour blind, though he can distinguish them on most parts of the retina which are not affected. As regards form vision, it was mentioned in the first chapter that in a healthy eye it is much more acute at the centre than towards the periphery, and instances were given of the angular distances apart that black dots on a white ground were required to be placed to allow their being seen as separate142 objects when the images were received on the centre of the retina, and at the periphery. Sharp definition may be said to be almost confined to 3° of angular distance at the centre, and most probably this is a happy state of affairs, for if we could see equally distinctly with the whole field of vision, the mind would be distracted from the object which it wished primarily to contemplate.

Bearing in mind the want of definition beyond 3°, and the indistinctness caused by a diseased central area, it will not be surprising to find that form vision in these cases is imperfect throughout, though the colour perception outside such area may be unimpaired. But, practically, men suffering from this disease are colour blind to coloured objects, such as a signal light on a railway or a ship’s light at sea. They may see that there is light at the distant signal or on the bow of a vessel, but will be unable to interpret correctly the colour. The colours which fail to make visual impressions are the reds and greens. Some will distinguish yellow, and very nearly all will distinguish blue with the centre of the eye. If a bright spectrum be thrown on the screen, and a tobacco-blind person be requested to name the colours of the different parts pointed out to him, it is often the case that as his eyes follow the143 pointer he will tell you that in the extreme red he sees no light, but in the bright red he sees dull white. The bright yellow he will tell you is a pale yellow or white, according as his case is a moderate or bad one; the green he will call white, and the blue and violet he will designate correctly. At the same time that his eye is turned away to another colour, he will see the true colour of the part of the spectrum which he has just incorrectly named, but it will disappear again as he turns his eyes back again. This tells us that his sense of colour is apparently unaffected outside the diseased area.

Fig. 35.

Left Eye.

Right Eye.

At page 10, a description has been given of the manner in which the field for colour and light has been determined, and if this same method be pursued with persons suffering from this form of colour blindness we get some remarkable results. Fig. 35 is the chart of the eye for red and for white, which was made by a case of tobacco blindness. The yellow spot is entirely affected, and, as is very common, it extends to the blind spot in the eye. At no place within that area can red be seen, though blue is immediately recognised. The extent of the field for white is that found under normal conditions, and except for the diseased area the same is true for the red. The144 fields for both eyes are given: that for the left eye in the left-hand chart, and that for the right eye in the right-hand chart. The small dark spots within the 5° area are places where the colour sensation is most defective. The part in the central dark area shaded with lines in this direction //// shows the portion of the field which is insensitive to red, though not to light, whilst the remainder of the shaded central area indicates the extent of the field which145 is sensitive to red. The field for light generally is also shown by the (approximately) rectangular unshaded area. Although the area occupied by the insensitive part of the retina is small compared with the whole, yet it is in that part which is used for distinct vision.

For testing for colour the apparatus, Fig. 3, arranged so that the patch of colour has the white patch alongside, is the most useful, but it is as well then to use a surface of patch about ½ inch square only, and thus to confine the image as nearly as may be to the spot on the retina which is defective. These cases of central scotoma are by no means very easy to test; for it frequently happens that before they are able to distinguish that there are two patches side by side, they have to approach very close to the screen. If this be the case, however, it will usually be found that the patches of ½ inch side are still efficient, as the near approach of the eyes to the screen indicates a wide area as being affected, so that the image still lies within the diseased retinal area. In some instances the colours named will vary very considerably; sometimes, for instance, a red will be named as grey, and then immediately after as pale red. This is generally due to the diseased area being small, and a very slight change in the direction of the axis of the eye causes146 it to be seen in nearly its true colour, part being viewed with the diseased and part with the healthy portion of the retina. With the wool test, which we shall describe later, it is the commonest thing possible for colour-blind persons who have a central scotoma to match accurately the different test-skeins, for the reason that the images of the skeins of wool are so large that they are received on the parts of the retina which are not diseased. These same colours, however, if presented to them in small patches, will inevitably show the defect in vision.

With this end in view, I have had a set of brick-clay pellets some 3/16-inch in diameter, painted with water-colours mixed with soluble glass solution of the same colours as the wools. These are placed in a shallow tray, and presented to patients affected with this central colour blindness to pick out all the pellets which match reds and greens. They will tell you that they see neither one nor the other, though they will pick out the blue pellets unerringly. A red pellet they will match with a red, green, grey, or a brown one, and a green one with the same. If, however, you instruct them to direct their eyes a few degrees away from the tray, they will tell you they see all the colours, and as they endeavour to pick them out,147 they, with a natural instinct, direct their eyes again to the collection, when once more the colours vanish. It is almost piteous sometimes to see the distress which this simple test occasions. The sight of the colours for an instant and their immediate disappearance in the cases that I have tried, seem indicative of something terrible, for they usually have no idea of the cause of this (to them almost miraculous) phenomenon. I have seen these colour blind tested with a pair of ordinary bull’s-eye lanterns, placed side by side, with diaphragms of moderate size with coloured glasses, which can be changed at will, in front. At twelve feet distance they will often see both lights as one, but as they approach they will make out two lights and call them both white, or sometimes they will make a guess and call a green red, or vice versâ. It goes without saying that such eyesight is useless for reading signals, and indeed for any purpose whatever. Sometimes, but I believe this is rare, no colour whatever can be distinguished.


148

CHAPTER XII.

I will now give in full the result of the examination of a patient who was suffering from tobacco blindness. X., aged thirty-six, a commercial traveller, was suffering from rather severe tobacco amblyopia. The scotoma was a very marked one, and the loss of colour sensation most complete. Mr. Nettleship, who furnished the case, has kindly added the following remarks on the case:—

His acuteness of vision was 6/36 with the right eye and 6/60 with the left. He smoked half-an-ounce of “shag” daily and drank about four pints of beer. His sight had been failing for about two months. As is common in early stages of this disease the ophthalmoscope revealed no decided changes at the optic discs.

He passed the test of the Holmgren wools satisfactorily, proving that the usual vision was normal for colour, but failed at once with the pellet test.

Fig. 36.

The objects in view were to test his perception of the spectrum colours, and then the extent of his retinal149 field for colour. This last is not recorded here. The spectrum colours were reduced to uniform luminosity150 between λ 4600 and λ 6600. Diaphragms containing holes of different sizes were placed in front of the last prism, and thus a round spot of monochromatic light of the same luminosity was produced upon the screen when a slit was passed through the spectrum. From the red end to λ 5270 he called the whole of the colours white, and from that point he began to see blue, called the colours bluish and blue. When the full illumination for all the colours was used, the same results were obtained. From this examination it would appear that he was totally deprived of the sensation of any colour except of blue. A subsequent examination of his perception of the luminosity of different rays, however, has to be taken into account, for in the first examination he had no light of pure white with which to compare the colours. In the next experiments, a strip of white light was placed in juxtaposition to the colour, and the results were slightly different. The table below gives his luminosity measures (Fig. 36). Col. I. is the empyric scale number, II. is the wave-length, III. the luminosity of the colour to the normal eye, IV. the luminosity to X., and V. the ratios of III. to IV.

In the diagram, his luminosity curve X. is shown, its area being 1400 against 1650 for the normal eye. His central perception of light, as arrived at by the extinction151 method, was only two-thirds of that of the normal eye; hence his area of luminosity should be 1100. As it is 1400, the ordinates of the above curve should be multiplied by 0·8, to compare with that of the normal eye.

I. II. III. IV. V. Colours to X. Spectrum
colour to
normal eye.
Scale
No.
Wave-
length.
Luminosity
to the
normal eye
Luminosity
to X.
IV.

III.
60 6730      7·3   0 0      Sees only the white stripe Red.
57 6423 32 10 0·31 Calls red yellowish, and white bluish Scarlet.
55 6242 65 38 0·65 „ „
53 6074 96 86 0·89 Both one colour Red-orange.
51 5920 99 90 0·91 „ „ Orange-yellow.
47 5660 92 83 0·90 Calls green a little blue; white he sees as white Greenish-yellow.
43 5430 69 625   0·90 „ „ Yellowish-green.
40 5270 50 46 0·92 „ „ Green.
32 4910      8·5   9 1·06 Sees blue as blue, and white yellowish Greenish-blue.
31 4960   7   8 1·14 „ „ Blue.
26 4680   3   3 1·00 „ „ Blue.

His readings of luminosity were made without any hesitation, and were concordant for each observation, which is not to be wondered at, as the matches, except152 at the blue end, were practically matches of different mixtures of black and white.

It appears that the white which X. sees as white is the same as the orange sodium light, and that the red he sees is yellowish. The mixture of this yellowish-white with the blue makes white. He sees a little blue in the spectrum colour at λ 5720, so it must be taken that at that point of the spectrum he begins to see colour—a point which is considerably lower than that given by his preliminary examination of the spectrum colour, and due, no doubt, to the fact that in this experiment he had the white light of the positive pole of the electric light to compare with it. It seems probable that what X. called yellowish was really a sensation of white mixed with a very small quantity of red sensation, for he saw no yellow in the orange, in which that colour would be most easily distinguished on account of its luminosity. Red light, when strongly diluted with white light, to the normal eye is often called orange.

As, practically speaking, the colour vision of X. is confined to blue and white, it is of interest to note the difference in luminosity at the different parts of the spectrum that is registered by him and by P., who had blue (violet) monochromatic vision. To facilitate the153 comparison, the luminosity curve of the latter is shown in the diagram.

Fig. 37.

The thin line curve is the normal curve.

154

Perhaps another case of a patient suffering from tobacco blindness may be quoted, as it will show the differences that exist in recognising the colours of the spectrum, and that the shorter the visible limit of the spectrum at the red end, the more pronounced is the extent of the colour blindness. G. suffered from a very well-marked tobacco scotoma, occupying a considerable area. His curve of luminosity of the spectrum is shown in Fig. 37. The horizontal band beneath will show the colours which the spectrum colours appeared to match.

Table of Luminosity for G. See page 153.

Scale
No.
Wave-
length.
Reading. Colours named by G. Colour of
spectrum to the
normal eye.
57 6423   0 Scarlet.
55 6242   3 No colour
53 6074 11 Colour “yellow,” white “blue” Red-orange.
51 5919 34 „ „ „ „ Orange-yellow.
50 5850 60 „ „ „ „
49 5783 64 Colour “gold,” white “sky-blue” Yellow.
45 5538 59
40 5270 40 Both white Green.
35 5042 18
30 4848 10
29 4807   6 Colour “very pale blue,” white as white Blue.
26 4707   4 Colour “blue,” white “white”
20 4518   3 „ „ „ „
10 4248   2 „ „ „ „ Violet.

155

G. was tested for light sense by the extinction method, and it appears that the final sensitiveness to light at the central part of the eye was nearly 12 times less than a person possessing normal sense. I may mention that I have examined one, if not two cases in which the patient was not only tobacco blind, but also congenitally colour blind. Though interesting for record, they need not be given in full here.

With these specimens of examination I must leave the cases of tobacco blindness. Although very important, they by no means constitute the sole cases of colour deficiency due to disease. I will give as an instance a case of loss of colour sensation due to progressive atrophy of both eyes which was examined, with Mr. Nettleship’s aid. When tested with spectrum colours—a patch of white light being placed in juxtaposition with the colour—it was found that W. S. was absolutely blind to colour from 26·75 (λ 4733) on the scale of the spectrum to the termination of the red of his spectrum, which was close to 63 on the scale (λ 7082). Above scale No. 26·75 W. S. saw blue, and his spectrum was continued normally in the violet. His luminosity curve (Fig. 37) was made without any difficulty, and,156 compared with my own, shows a slight deficiency in brightness from the red to the yellow, but his perception of luminosity increases as the blue is approached.

Table of Luminosity for W. S. See page 155.

Scale
No.
Wave-
length.
Reading. Spectrum colours
named by W. S.
Spectrum
colours to
normal eye.
60 6728   3·4 Grey Scarlet.
58 6520 15·0
56 6330 41·0
55 6242 43   
54 6152 69   
52 5996 94   
50 5850 100      Orange.
48 5720 96   
45 5538 88   
42 5373 74   
40 5270 61·5 Green.
38 5172 45   
35 5042 30   
30 4848 12    Blue.
25 4675 Bluish
20 4518
15 4376 Blue Violet.
10 4248   2·5

He was subsequently tested with colour discs—Ultra-marine (U), Red-royal (R), Emerald-green (G), Chrome-yellow (Y), White (W), and Black (B).

157

It was found that—

165 (U) + 48 (R) + 147 (G) = 75 (W) + 285 (B).

The black reflected 3·4 of white; hence the true equation is—

 (i). 165 (U) + 48 (R) + 147 (G) = 84·7 (W) + 275 (B).

(ii). 120 (U) + 240 (Y) = 196 (W) + 164 (B) (corrected)—

With 260 (U) + 100 (Y) he sees blue.
 250 (U) + 110 (Y) he sees light-blue.
 242 (U) + 118 (Y) he sees no blue.

This last in connection with (ii) shows that his blue perception is neutralised by the yellow, although the yellow to him was matched with white.

I have already shown you a chart of the insensitive area of the retina found in a tobacco-blind case, and it may be advisable that you should see an example of the curtailment that exists, both for light and colour, in the field of vision of eyes in which there is progressive atrophy of the optic nerves. The large black area shows the part of the field that was encroached upon. The dark spots show small areas which are also insensitive. The field for colour shown by the inner shaded area is also encroached upon, and practically the patient was blind in a great part of his field. His form vision was also very bad, and his colour perception feeble. The three charts given158 in these lectures were brought by Mr. Nettleship, for the information of the Colour Vision Committee of the Royal Society, and by his permission they are reproduced here.

Fig. 38.

Left Eye.

Right Eye.

Two other cases I may give in some detail, one in which the sensation of colour is totally absent in the left eye, the right eye being normal; and the other in which there is colour blindness of a very rare character. The first case is that of a lady, whom we will call Miss W. It appears from159 the history of this lady that she had a slight stroke of paralysis which affected her left side, and that she subsequently found her left eye was deprived of all sensation of colour. It is said by the specialists who examined her retina that this is a case of atrophy of the optic nerve. She had very little difficulty in matching the most brilliant spectrum colours with the white patch of light. Her curve of luminosity is given in Fig. 39 (see table, page 228). At 19 of the scale, which is well in the blue, she had very little sense of light, though her extinction curve shows that it extended to some distance beyond. The eye in which normal vision existed was, during the examination of the defective eye, bound up with a handkerchief, and when occasionally she was allowed to use both eyes, her astonishment was great to see the colours which she had matched with the white. The curve of luminosity taken with her right eye coincided with my own, which throughout we have taken as normal. From her extinction curve we gather that there was a marked diminution of sensitiveness to light in her left eye compared with that of normal vision. Apparently, in that eye she only has 1/25 of the normal sensitiveness to light near E in the green, but her extinction curve takes the same general form as that160 of the normal eye. The difference between the sets of ordinates of the two indicates the difference in sensitiveness for each part of the spectrum.

Fig. 39.

Her persistency curve as calculated occupies the same position and is of about the same dimensions, when the161 maximum is made 100, as that of the normal eye, as it is therefore of red- and green-blind, and also of162 the two cases of monochromatic vision. We have in Miss W. a type of colour blindness which no present theory of colour vision accounts for without straining; and it would probably have to refer it to the seat of sensation rather than to the retina alone.

Fig. 40.

The thin line curve is the curve of luminosity for the normal eye.

The second is a case of congenital colour blindness and with no trace of disease, brought by Mr. Nettleship to the same Committee. He found that this lady, N. W., mistook blue for red, and it was with some curiosity that this case was examined. Her first examination was as to colour sense with the spectrum colours, a patch of monochromatic light being placed in juxtaposition with an equal patch of white light. At 62·5 (λ 6890) of the scale the light of the spectrum disappeared. As the slit moved along the spectrum, and the white was approximately reduced to equal luminosity, she described all the red as grey, and of the same colour as the white until 53·5 (λ 6110). At this point she said the colour was brownish compared with the white, and this hue continued to her till 48 on the scale (λ 5720), when she said the colour was “neither brown nor green, but both.” From 48 on the scale she described the colour as green, when it changed quite suddenly at 31·5 (λ 4905). From this point and in the blue she again began to see grey; the grey at this end of the spectrum, and also of the white patch, she called brownish-grey. This name must evidently have been a mental distinction, as she described the red end and the white as grey only, and not brownish-grey; and, indeed, she was tested again over that part of the spectrum, and adhered to the previous naming. It would appear to be due to low luminosity, which made the grey appear to her what she called brownish, rather than to any actual difference in hue.

163

Scale
No.
Wave-
length.
Reading. Colours named by N. W. Spectrum colours
to normal vision.
60 6728   3 Both grey Red.
58 6520 10  
56 6330 30  
54 6152 52 Colour “brownish,” white “grey”
52 5996 70 „ „ „
50 5850 81 „ „ „ Orange.
48 5720 87 Colour “brownish-green.” white “grey”
46 5596 90 Colour “green,” white “grey”
44 5481 88 „ „  
42 5373 82 „ „  
40 5270    62·5 „ „ Green.
38 5172 46 „ „  
35 5042 23 „ „  
32 4924    12·5 „ „  
31 4886 10 Colour “brownish-grey,” white “brownish-green.”
   30·5 4862      8·5 „ „ „ Blue.
25 4675   5 „ „ „  
20 4518   3 „ „ „  
15 4376      2·5 „ „ „  
10 4248      1·5 „ „ „ Violet.
0 4010      0·2 „ „ „  

164

Her curve of luminosity in the spectrum was next taken, and her readings are given in the table above. The curve is shown in Fig. 40. The shaded band beneath it applies to her curve. Miss W.’s luminosity curve is also repeated in the same figure for the sake of comparison.

An endeavour was made to form a series of colour equations with her eyesight by placing three slits in different parts of the spectrum, but without success, although a match with white was made in two positions. One slit was in the orange-red (52 of the scale), another at E, and the third at G; mixtures were made which she said matched the white, but they were so erratic that it was useless to measure the apertures. When the slit in the violet was covered up, a white patch being alongside as a comparison, she called the mixture of red and green “brownish-green”; when the slit in the red was covered she called the mixed165 light of green and violet “green”; and when the green slit was covered up she called the purple colour a “different kind of brown.”

When the first slit was moved into the red near the lithium line she called the colours “green,” whenever the green slit was uncovered. A piece of red glass was placed in the white reflected beam, forming a red patch, and a patch of the blue at scale No. 30·5 (λ 4862) was placed alongside, and she matched them in luminosity and in colour. (The dominant colour of the signal glass in question was λ 6220.) She finally was tested with colour discs.

To make white she required

130 G + 113 R + 117 U = 72 W + 218 B.

She was then tried with the blue and green discs alone and made a match—

258 U + 102 G = 65 W + 295 B.

An attempt was made to match with the green and red discs alone, but this failed.

She matched the red disc alone with black and white, and also the blue disc alone—

360 R = 56 W + 304 B (corrected),
360 U = 60 W + 300 B (corrected).

With any proportion of R and U mixed together she matched a grey of approximately the same intensity as166 above, as it might be supposed she would from the last two equations.

Taking the intensity curve of the light reflected from the red disc, it was found to contain a great deal of the part of the spectrum which she called brownish, viz., from 33·5 to 48 on the scale, whereas the blue reflected a trifle of this portion of the spectrum, as did also the green; and this may account for her making a match to grey of U and G, and not of R and G, but it is hard to see why she matched U alone and also R with the grey.

Reviewing the case, it seems that any perception of colour is very small, and that the sensations are green and much less red. From the equations it also seems that she would have matched green with white and black alone, and that 360 G = 75 W + 285 B. Perhaps the explanation of the matches and names of colours may be that a proportion of colour may be mixed with another without being perceived, but this colour so hidden has still the capability of neutralising a certain quantity of the complementary colour.


167

CHAPTER XIII.

You have been taken through much experimental work, and possibly it may be thought that there has been too much of it; but now that we are coming to the more practical part of the subject, it will become apparent that a good working hypothesis is absolutely necessary before effectual tests for colour vision can be carried out, and that the reasons for its adoption should be given in full. The question of colour blindness is one of very practical importance, as in certain occupations it is essential that colours should be accurately and quickly known, and that no guess-work should be allowed. Lives have without doubt been lost by a want of proper knowledge of colours, both at sea and on railways. The evidence that such is the case is, as a rule, it is true, merely negative, though there are cases extant where great losses which have occurred can be traced to a deficiency in colour perception. If there be no proper system of tests for ascertaining the defects of168 signal or look-out men in their colour sense, it is palpable that positive evidence cannot be forthcoming, and this is very much the state of things which exists up to the present time. We hear of collisions at sea and vessels foundering in consequence of the rule of the road not being followed, but at the investigations which follow we have no record that the question of colour perception of the look-out man has been gone into, though there may be conflicting evidence as to whether a red light or a green light was shown. That danger from colour blindness is incurred has for some time been recognised by the Board of Trade, as it insists that all officers of the Mercantile Marine must be tested for their sense of colour, and that their certificates must be endorsed as having failed to pass the colour test should they do so. For my own part, I think endorsement of their certificate is quite inadequate, for it is still open for shipowners to employ them (of course at their own risk). A rejection for colour vision should entail a withholding of the certificate altogether; for it surely is as dangerous that a signal should be misread as it is that the logarithm of the sine of an angle should be misunderstood. If a candidate fails in theoretical navigation, he is not allowed a certificate, but if he only fails in a very169 practical part of his examination, his certificate is merely endorsed.

The system employed by this department was a defective one, and we know of many instances in which candidates have passed the colour test, though they ought to have been rejected, and are at present in the service. The subject of testing for colour vision was brought prominently forward some two or three years ago, and a Committee of the Royal Society, to which I acted as secretary, was requested to consider the methods at that time in force on the railways and in the mercantile marine, and to find one which was not open to objection. It recommended the system that had been elaborated by Holmgren, a Swedish physicist, and known as Holmgren’s test, which has long been in force in Sweden and elsewhere. This system has, I am glad to say, been adopted by the Board of Trade, and by most of the railway companies in the United Kingdom. There have been numerous indications that this change of method was necessary. Only within the last month (April, 1894), for instance, I was informed by the Medical Officer who had to examine the employés on a certain railway in Scotland by the Holmgren test that he had found some, amongst others an engine-driver, who were colour blind, and presumably170 unfit for the posts they occupied owing to this defect.

There is one popular objection which is always made against this test, or indeed against any proper test, viz., that the examination is not made under the same conditions which absolutely exist, nor with the very lights which the candidates have to distinguish from one another—that is, the red and green lights. Let me beg of you to remark, that as a mere matter of guessing, the chances are equal that a man would name the light shown correctly. If you turn a man’s back to the light, and if he has a coin in his pocket and deliberately calls heads red and tails green, he will have a good chance of passing the test; for, if he guessed rightly three or four times, no one would fail to pass him on his answers. The great point in a test is to cause the candidate to do something to show that he appreciates colour. It is this doing something and saying nothing which is the important feature in the Holmgren test. A man may be ignorant of the names of colours—colour ignorant it is called—but he cannot be ignorant of the colours themselves if he has normal colour vision. As a matter of fact, the colour blind may possibly distinguish between red and green lights by having carefully noted, under ordinary conditions171 of atmosphere, their different brightness, and by their difference in saturation with their neutral colour. If external conditions are altered, as they are in actual daily life, these slight indications vanish, and the quick naming of the colour to be read becomes a mere matter of chance. A proper test should include all variations that can occur in these respects. It cannot be too strongly impressed upon every one that a man who is colour blind to colours in ordinary daylight is equally so in lamplight, although some shades of colour which are well distinguishable by daylight may disappear when the artificial light is used as the source of illumination.

Now, on what scientific principles should a colour test be founded? We must hark back to a theory for a moment, and as it has been shown that for all essential purposes that of Young answers, we will use it as a good working hypothesis, and it was from this theory that Holmgren himself reasoned. The red- or green-blind see a grey in a part of their spectrum, which to us who possess normal colour vision is green. If then we present such a green to them, they would match it with a grey. If, however, we have a yellowish-green, which is pure green mixed with red, the complete green-blind will not see the green in it, but172 only the red. The colour to him would be very pale red, and as he sees all such greens and yellows and reds as red more or less saturated, that is, more or less mixed with his neutral colour, any one of these he would match with a green. The red-blind, on the other hand, would see all these colours as green, and he too might make similar matches with them. Suppose now we have a pink skein: the green-blind would see it as a white or bluish-white, for a purple is white to him, and he would match with it either greys or colours having a slight excess of blue in them; for a green is to him a neutral colour. The red-blind, on the other hand, would see but little green in the pink; blue would predominate, so he would choose mauves or blues amongst other matches.

Acting on these principles, Holmgren selected his test colours. He chose wools as the most convenient for handling, and also because they present the same colour without sheen when looked at in any direction. His first test colour is a very pale green which contained no blue. Its paleness is a point in its favour. The colour is quite distinguishable by us normal-visioned persons, but it might appear as grey to the red- and green-blind; for as we who possess normal vision may mix a small percentage of colour with our neutral colour173 (white) without it being perceived, so may they with theirs (white and green). As the green, when it is to us saturated, would be nearly neutral coloured to them, the very diluted colour which we see in the skein would to them be masked by the addition of white. In any case, if any colour be visible to them, it must be on the red side of the neutral points. A candidate is given this skein of wool, and from a heap of over a hundred skeins, of varying degrees of saturation, amongst which are drabs, yellows, yellow-greens, blue-greens, purples, pinks, greys, and so on, he is asked to select others which appear to him to be of the same colour as the test-skein, though they may be darker or lighter. He will, if colour blind, select some of the colours already indicated. The second test-skein is a pink, which is a purple diluted with white, but much less so than the green, to which it is nearly a complementary in daylight. The candidate is required to select colours which match this, and according to his selections is he pronounced as having normal colour vision or as being colour defective (either completely or partially) to the red or to the green. The case of violet blindness is not important in reading the signals ordinarily used, and therefore in this test no special test-skein is employed. Let us consider what colour we174 should use. The neutral colour to this form of colour blindness is yellow. If, therefore, we pick out a pale yellow skein, the candidate would pick out greys to match it; or if we gave him the pink skein to match, since he has no blue (violet) sensation, he would match it with a pure red or with a purple.

Where monochromatic vision is under examination, all skeins would be matched with one another indiscriminately—blues, reds, greens, greys will all be a match, some lighter and some darker than the test-skein. I have been told by some who have carried out examinations for colour blindness that this matching is by no means so uncommon as is often imagined. In future it is hoped that most of those who make these matches may be examined by the spectrum test, as it may turn out that a proportion of them will be most valuable theoretical cases.

In making an examination with the Holmgren test, it is almost unnecessary that the candidate should take up a skein out of the heap of wools to form a preliminary diagnosis. The colour blind will not at once pick out an evident match, but will hesitate and evince a desire to appear very accurate in his choice. This indicates at once that there is something amiss. He probably will pick up a skein of the right175 colour, place it against the test-skein, lay it down and again take it up. Or he will pick up a skein which is evidently incorrect and do the same thing, but perhaps he will return it to the heap and take up another which is equally bad.

He will fumble over making his matches, and eventually have a heap by him which will at once tell the examiner that he is colour defective. I may as well give you an idea of the colours which the colour blind will pick out by a simple experiment. The heap of wools is on the table, and in the pure white of the electric arc light, which is thrown on it from the lantern, every colour is distinct in hue and in intensity. On one side are placed the two important test-skeins, the pale green and the pink. There can be no doubt but that in that heap of wools there are a large number which can be matched with each of them. The red-blind, be it recollected, sees no red, and if I can place in front of the lens of the lantern some medium which cuts off the red as completely as possible, the audience as well as myself will see the colours approximately as the red-blind would do. Such a medium is found in the same blue-green glass that is used for signals on most railways and on board ship. The green-blind, on the other hand, see no green, and if176 a medium can be found which when placed in the path of the light allows no green to pass, the colours in the heap being deprived of the green would be such as would very nearly be the same as this type of colour blind would see. This glass is covered with a film of collodion in which fuschin and blue have been dissolved. It transmits a fine purple and should answer our purpose. That these two media are what we require can be readily demonstrated by placing them in front of the slit of the collimator of our colour apparatus and throwing the spectrum on the screen. The spectrum of white light is now on the screen, and when we place the blue-green glass in front of the slit, we see that the red is very nearly entirely extinguished, whilst if we substitute for it the dyed collodionized glass the green is absent. Now, placing the first glass in front of the lantern lens and switching on the current, the wools are illuminated with the bluish-green light. The green test-skein appears green, and we can proceed to make our matches, picking out the colours which appear the same, but taking no heed as to their lightness or darkness. A dozen skeins are now picked out, and I think the audience will agree with me that the matches as viewed in the green light are accurate. The glass is now withdrawn, and the ordinary white light falls upon the skeins177 in my hand. They are a strangely variegated lot as now seen; we have green shades, yellows, and browns, and greys. Such a variety would tell me that I was colour deficient, but would not be, perhaps, decisive as to what was the exact character of the deficiency. For if the pink glass is placed in front of the lantern you will find the same matches, with one or two exceptions, might have been made. The blue-green glass is once more placed in the beam, and this time I match the pink skein with the wools. A certain number are picked out, and the audience will agree with me that the matches are fair ones. When, however, the glass is withdrawn from the light and we see what colours have been selected, we find that they consist of pale blues, mauves, pinks of various shades, and cerise, and violet. The red in the pink did not affect my eyes any more than would it the red-blind. I am evidently then in this light red-blind, for if the pink glass replaces the blue-green, the matches are impossible. While this coloured light is illuminating the heap I will make matches again. When made, the white light is again thrown on the selected skeins, and this time we have bluish-green and neutral tint together with pinks. The reason of this is evident, there is no green visible; the bluish-green contains besides blue a certain amount of yellow,178 which, in its turn, contains red, and the grey must be pink. To the green-blind, for reasons already given, the blue-green looks white, as does the pink, and therefore the two are matched together. The grey is also degraded white to him, and therefore he also matches that with them. The matches which the violet-blind would make can be well exemplified by placing in the beam of light a yellow glass, or a glass coated with collodion in which “brilliant yellow” has been dissolved. By this plan, then, we can in some measure produce the effect of colour blindness on ourselves, and very interesting it is to compare theory with the results obtained in this manner. There is no necessity to have recourse to the electric light for this purpose. If matches are made with such media held before the eyes in ordinary daylight, the same results will be obtained. I have often examined through these same media the matches made by the colour blind, and been able at once to settle the nature of the defective vision from which they were suffering. It must be remembered that the colours transmitted through these two glasses are not absolutely like the whites which the two classes of colour blind see respectively, though they approach it.

We can imitate even more exactly the matches that179 they would make by matching white light with a mixture of red, green, and violet of the proper hues, and covering up the red or green slit, and then placing the test-skein and the matches in the colour so formed. From the other skeins viewed in the same light can be picked out the matches which would be possible. There is very little chance, if any, of a mistake about them being made when this plan is adopted.


180

CHAPTER XIV.

Holmgren’s test, although a qualitative one, is most accurate in allowing a diagnosis to be formed, but it sometimes happens that a candidate is not satisfied that he has failed in passing the test, and wishes for another examination. Such a re-examination is best carried out by the spectrum method, which I will now describe.

The test with the spectrum is a very decisive one, and can be carried out with the patch apparatus (Fig. 3), page 19. Personally, I like to have some idea of the kind of colour blindness, if any, which exists by first using the Holmgren test. Should these tests show that a candidate is colour blind in any degree, a very excellent beginning is to try and find his neutral point in the spectrum—if he has one. To arrive at it we place two patches of light on the screen, one of colour and the other of white, the rotating sectors being in the last-named beam, and ask him to say when the two colours appear alike. It must be remembered that white is coloured from the effect of contrast as long as the181 colour alongside differs from it. A good point de depart is with the slit in the yellow, then to move it into the red, and then gradually to push it into the green. When here, if colour blind, he will say, “The two patches are nearly alike, but that the white is rather pink or green,” as the slit gets further towards the blue. The operator, whilst changing the colour, alters the sectors so that the luminosities are about the same. A point will be reached when the colour blind will say, “Now they are both alike, but one is rather darker than the other.” The sectors are altered until he says they are both alike, and the observation is satisfactory when he declares the two patches of light are both alike in colour and in darkness. It is curious how misleading the word brightness is to some people who are uneducated. I find it much safer to ask which is the darker colour, rather than which is the brighter. A little patience will always enable you to get a good observation. The place in the spectrum which is the neutral point is now noted. The neutral point is again found, but this time commencing in the blue. The same procedure is adopted as before, and we thus get a second reading for it, and the two will be found to be very close to one another. In difficult cases, four or five observations may be182 made, and the mean taken as a close approximation. So far the spectrum test has not shown whether the observer is red- or green-blind, except by comparing the position of the neutral point with that usually found by the two types. We have, however, an unerring criterion by the luminosity method. The red is placed beside the white, and he is asked to say which he considers the darker; he will give an answer of some kind, and probably protest that the two colours are not alike. A soothing answer will disarm his objection, and he will quickly see what you mean. If he be red-blind he will match in brightness a brilliant red and a feeble white; if he be green-blind he will make a match very similar to normal vision. In the case of the red-blind the slit is then moved into the extreme red, when he will say he sees but one patch of light, whilst the green-blind will see it as a person of normal vision would do. If time permits, the whole luminosity curve may be taken and registered. This is not essential, but interesting for reference. Where complete colour blindness exists, it should be possible to cause him to match a green with a red. To do this a second instrument, as described in page 18, may be used, but it is quite sufficient if a piece of red glass, such as is used for railway signals, or of183 bottle-green glass, be placed in the white beam. There is then a red or green patch alongside the patch of spectrum colour. The red will stimulate the red sensation of the green-blind, but not being spectrum red it contains a certain amount of yellow, which stimulates the green sensation if the observer be red-blind. The green is of such a colour that it will stimulate both the red and the green sensations. In the path of the reflected beam between G and the prisms (Fig. 3) a sheet of plain glass is inserted, which reflects a proportion of white on to the red patch. The sectors are placed in this beam. If the red glass is being used, the slit is moved into the green near E, and the colour blind will say that both are the same colour, but one darker than the other. By opening or closing the slit in the spectrum, he will possibly say that both colours are alike and of the same darkness, but he may say one is paler than the other, in which case the white light must be increased or diminished by means of the sectors till equality of tone is established. This applies to the red-blind and the green-blind. The former will require a very bright red to match a feeble green, whilst with the latter the red will require a fairly light green. When the green glass is used the spectrum colour patch should be red, and the match be made as before. With184 the violet-blind the neutral point will be in the yellow, and with monochromatic vision matches can be made throughout the spectrum. So far it will be seen that no mention of any colour is required. It may next be advisable to ask him the names of colours. This is best done by placing the white patch of light over the spectrum colour patch, and opening and closing, as may be required, the sectors. If the sectors are closed it is very probable that correct guesses may be made, for then the colours will be saturated, and the colour blind, if they are intelligent, will know that a green to them is white or pale in colour compared with red, though of the same hue. If white be mixed with the red the wrong name is bound to be given, for they will be unable to distinguish it from the green, because it is then a less saturated colour. Passing from green to red and mixing the colour more or less with white, the most—I was going to say grotesque—telling mistakes are made. A further excellent test is to place a cell containing a solution of bichromate in the path of the reflected beam, and cause the observer to match its colour with the light coming through two slits, one in the red near C, and the other in the green near E. Defective colour perception will be well demonstrated. There are various other artifices which can be employed185 in the spectrum test, which would be too long to recount here, and if there be two sets of apparatus the tests are practically unlimited in number.

There are cases in which an observer who may have normal vision may wish to be reported as colour blind. A seaman’s life is not always a happy one, and a boy on a training-ship, knowing that a failure in colour vision will free him from a sea life, may be anxious to be told he has failed in colour vision. By “coaching” in the Holmgren test he might manage to obtain a “failure,” but a malingerer is sure to be detected by the spectrum method of testing. He may call diluted red green, and he may declare he sees a neutral point in the spectrum, but if he be tested with the diluted colours near his supposed neutral point, he is sure to fall into a trap. He will make a mistake in calling a patch green when it ought to be white, or white when it ought to be green, if he were truly colour deficient—indeed, a malingerer has no chance of escaping detection with the spectrum tests. It is not an uninteresting experiment to get an acute observer who has normal colour vision, and is accustomed to the spectrum test, to feign colour blindness, and examine him in this manner. He never fails to make such mistakes as would lead to his detection.

186

With the partially colour blind the same procedure may be adopted. In examination by the Holmgren wool test, slight mistakes will be made in matching the first two test-skeins. With the spectrum test the red will require a greater dilution with white before it will be matched with a green, even if it can be matched at all. Measures of the luminosity at four or five positions in the spectrum, extending from near the extreme red to the blue, will give an unerring criterion of the kind and extent of colour blindness from which they are suffering. The existence of a neutral point in the spectrum is sufficient to indicate that their blindness is of a nature to be dangerous in certain occupations. To some it may be a difficulty how a neutral point can be found in such cases, since all sensations are more or less present. The reason, however, was explained on page 96.


187

CHAPTER XV.

Examples of colour blindness have been brought to your notice, and various measurements made by persons possessing normal and defective colour vision have been recorded, but no attempt has been made to discuss the two leading rival theories that have been laid before you. Regarding these theories you may expect me to say something, and to avow myself a partisan of one or the other. This last I must decline to do, though it will have been seen by the line that I have taken in these lectures that the Young theory attracts me. There are, however, difficulties in adapting it to explain several facts of colour vision which seem to render it, to say the least, incomplete. For instance, to explain the colours produced by simultaneous contrast, the Young theory has to betake itself into psychological ground. I will show you some excellent examples of contrast colours. We have upon the screen a patch of white reflected light, superposed over a patch of red188 light. Placing a thin rod in the paths of the two beams, we have two shadows—one illuminated by white and the other by red, and lying between them a mixed light of red and white. The shadow illuminated by the white does not appear white, but a bluish-grey. When the spectrum colour is changed to orange the blue is intensified, whilst when it is green, what should be white appears of an orange-salmon colour. Other colours give the white different hues which I need not describe.

These contrast colours are usually said to be complementary to the spectrum colours employed, though it must be recollected that what a complementary colour should be is determined by the quality of the white light which the two, when mixed, are made to match. But recent measures of my own show that they are not truly complementary in most instances, whatever the white light may be. But whether they are or not does not much matter when the explanation offered by the followers of the Young theory is considered, for it is asserted that such contrast colours have no real existence, but are psychological, or—what this comes to be—simply delusions. If they are not real colours felt by the retina, they have a very good resemblance to them, and the same series of delusions are so persistent and so189 constant for all normal vision that they can always be measured as having a constant value. I bear in mind the experiment in which the contrast colour, after being produced, is isolated in the eye from the colour producing it and the background, and the continuance of the hue produced by the contrast. This retention may be psychological, but there are no grounds to my mind for saying that its production is due to the same cause, more especially as experiments have been arranged to show that one eye may see a contrast colour, whilst the other may see it of its uncontrasted hue. In this last experiment it can scarcely be conceived that one eye should be subject to delusion, whilst the other was free from it. If, then, we may presume that they are real colours, the Young theory fails to explain them, and the explanation offered by the Hering theory is much more acceptable, as it propounds the idea that the retina has to be considered as a whole, and that if (say) red light is at work at one part its complementary colour (blue-green) must be felt at another. It would be still more acceptable had it happened that the contrast colours were truly complementary, and if the same action was noticeable when the adjacent part of the retina was not also stimulated.

For what I may call the straightforward part of190 colour vision, dealing with ordinarily bright colours, the Young theory is amply sufficient; but when we come to the feeble luminosities and the colour fields, it is again difficult to adapt to explain the phenomena observed. When we reduce the luminosity of a coloured ray sufficiently we feel the sensation of grey light: no colour is felt. Why is this? On the Hering theory it is capable of the explanation that we have the white sensation left unextinguished, but I fail to see any explanation on the Young theory. When we take colour fields with pure colours (see appendix, page 208), we are met with the unexplained difficulty that the colour from a bright spot of light vanishes almost suddenly towards the periphery of the retina, and is replaced by a bright white light, and that the extent of the field depends on the brightness of the colour. This, perhaps, is the most telling observation which can be recorded against the Young theory as it stands at present. It has this support, however, in the sequence of the phenomena observed, viz., when the boundary for the colour which we will suppose to be pure red is being taken (as described at page 11), that close to the point where it bursts into pure white, it assumes a pink colour (i.e., a mixture of red and white), whilst, if the red be scarlet, containing according191 to this theory a little green sensation, it becomes orange before white, showing that the red sensation is dimmed slightly before the green, and so with the other colours. What are called “after images” I have not touched upon so far, nor shall I here, for it is at this point that we step into very debateable ground. The colours perceived in them are, as yet, not capable of being put to the test of physical measurement, and I must leave the psychologist or the physiologist to account for them in their own way.

Viewing the Hering theory from a physical standpoint, and in the light of colour measurement, it appears to be deficient in several respects. To take one point. We have seen that when blue and yellow are mixed together to make white the sum of the luminosities of the two colours separately is equal to the luminosity of the white produced. According to the Hering theory, the yellow colour contains a certain amount of the white-black sensation besides the yellow sensation, as does also the blue colour besides the blue sensation. The theory tells us that when white is produced by the mixture, the blue sensation undoes the work that the yellow sensation has done, and the white sensation is alone left behind. If this be the case, the sum of the separate luminosities cannot192 be the same as that of the white produced, but should be greater. The theory also has to be strained sometimes to make it fit in with other observed facts. Take, for instance, the case of persons who are called red-blind and green-blind on the Young theory. We are told by the Hering theory that both are red-green-blind—that is, blind to both green and red, and only see blue and yellow—and that the only difference between them is that the former has his spectrum slightly shortened at the red end, the maxima of the yellow-blue sensations being shifted a little further towards the violet end of the spectrum. The natural question to ask is: Why this shift occurs? Surely it is more rational to adopt a theory which does not require such a supposition? If the sensitive matter acted upon by the yellow-blue rays be always of the same chemical composition, the shift cannot occur. It might, perhaps, be allowed that one shift was practicable, but, unfortunately, the shifts must become numerous when the cases of partial colour blindness are to be accounted for, and this would necessitate a constantly varying chemical composition of this matter, and of that acted upon by the red-green rays.

Again, in the extinction of the spectrum, the red and the green sensations in quantities to neutralize one193 another should be extinguished nearly together, even allowing for what physiologists tell us is the case, that the breaking down, or dissimulation, of cell tissue continues longer than its building up, but we find a large difference between the two. As already indicated, the luminosity curve of the feeble spectrum favours the theory of Hering being that here we only have the white-black sensation, and naturally the persistency curves must be scored in its favour. But the cases of B. C. and M., it seems to me, cannot be explained by the theory without any undue straining or assumptions. If we try and fit the cases of colour blindness due to tobacco scotoma to the theory, we find that in many cases yellow is not recognised, though blue is invariably. If the blue be active, the yellow should also be so.

And here I may remark that it has been assumed that the two classes of colour blindness are due to different causes. A question to ask ourselves is whether all colour blindness may not have been caused originally by disease. In the congenital form, it is true, no disease of the retina is traceable in the eye, and it is usually hereditary, but it does not follow that the want of response of the perceiving apparatus to certain sensations may not have been due to what, for want of a better expression, I may call an hereditary partial194 paralysis of the perceiving apparatus. If this be so, we have a connecting link between the two classes, and then a perfect theory should explain both classes on the same grounds. The suspicion that the monochromatic vision of P. and Q. might possibly be due to disease before birth, owing to the behaviour of their eyes under certain conditions, would then be explicable. I have no desire to press this view, though it seems to me to be one which is not out of all reason, taking analogies from other defects which are hereditary.

It has been usually accepted that the fields for blue and yellow in the eye are approximately the same, as are those of the green and red, and this has been taken as showing the interdependence between the two pairs according to the Hering theory. It has already been pointed out that the question of extent of fields requires still further investigation beyond that which it has received, and measures made by the method given on page 208 seem to cast a doubt as to whether this interdependence can be upheld. It will be noticed that the fields do not extend proportionately on the nasal and temporal sides (see also Fig. 3). It should also be remarked that the order of extent of field for the different colours does not follow the same order as their disappearance. A point that is195 sometimes raised in favour of Hering’s theory is the negative image formed after the eye is fatigued by looking at bright red or bright green. The negative images (see page 30) are said to be the complementary of these colours. The Young theory tells us that the red or the green sensation suffers fatigue by one or other colour, and that when the eye subsequently rests on a grey surface the other two sensations are chiefly stimulated and cause the complementary colour. It is said that it is easier to produce a negative green image than a negative red image, and the adherents of Hering tell us that this is due to the fact that destructive action is more readily carried out than constructive. In the Young theory, it is held that the green sensation is always mixed with white, whilst the red is fairly pure, and thus, for equal luminosities, the surplus green sensation is much less stimulated than the red, which offers a consistent explanation of this fact. There are several other minor difficulties in the way of accepting Hering’s theory as it stands from a physical point of view, but we need not discuss them now.

The final sensation curves for the spectrum colours on the Young theory are still under consideration, and are not definitely fixed, though the observations made have been very numerous. Recently Helmholtz, in the196 last edition of his “Physiological Optics,” has calculated, from Kœnig’s observations, that no one of the three sensations is singly stimulated by any colour, even at the extreme ends of the spectrum, and he makes the three fundamental sensations vary considerably from those given in these pages. Every colour he states is considerably mixed with white light. The calculations by which he arrived at this conclusion are of a complicated nature, and I think if he had had besides the colour equations of Kœnig, the luminosities and the extinction measures before him, there might have been a modification of his views, for these last give evidence to the contrary.

There is a possible modification of the Young theory which would account for a good many of the phenomena that are unaccounted for by it in its present form, though it may raise new difficulties in the minds of some. Let us suppose that each of the three sensations were compounded of fundamental light and of colour in fixed and definite proportions, and not in the same proportion in each; and further that the apparatus in the eye which was responsible for each sensation had two functions, one of which was to respond to the fundamental light sensation and the other to the colour. One essential difference between197 this modification of the Young theory and that of Hering is that, whilst in the latter the white sensation is a sensation distinct from the colour sensations, in the former it is a definite part of them. The fact that the sensation of colour is lost before the sensation of light is one of the greatest significance, and any theory to be accepted must offer a reasonable explanation of it. If the modification suggested be made, it accounts for the existence of this residuum of light equally as well as Hering’s theory, and without its drawback. It is not hard to imagine the apparatus which gives rise to two sensations, on the assumption of different kinds of atomic motion, induced by the ether motion, or at least three kinds are possible. When extinction of colour is made, the ether vibrations would have sufficient energy to induce but one kind of motion; and when all light was extinguished from the same ray, they would not be capable of inducing any sensible motion whatever. In the case of Miss W., who saw all colours as white, it might be that disease had entirely prevented the first kind of motion in all three sensations, and that in P. and Q. the red and green sensations were absent or paralysed in their entirety, whilst the blue sensation was left in full operation. In B. C. the blue and red sensations would be similarly absent, leaving198 the green sensation unchanged. The coincidence of their persistency and luminosity curves would then indicate that the proportions of fundamental light and colour remained the same throughout. Other examples and considerations seem to indicate that the proportion of colour to fundamental light is greatest in the red sensation, next in the green, and least in the blue. This would explain why with increasing intensities blue appears white sooner than green, and much sooner than red. The proposed modification would also offer the necessary explanation as to the disappearance of colour from the field.

Looking at colour vision from what I may call an evolutionary point of view, the “light-colour” theory commends itself as probable. There are many reasons for thinking that the visual sensation first evolved was that of light, subsequently followed by that of colour. The first evolved colour sensation would appear to have been the blue, and the last the red. The discussion of this hypothesis would carry me beyond my limits, and I must leave it thus baldly expressed for your consideration.

For my own part, whatever theory of colour sensations may prove to be the right one, I lean strongly to the idea that the cause of vision will be found in chemical199 action, induced by the impact of the different wave-lengths of light falling on sensitive matter. A white substance may absorb all the wave-lengths found in the spectrum, and if it have three sets of molecules, one of which has an atom or atoms vibrating with the same period as the waves of light which show a maximum for one sensation and another for another, and so on, the requirements for the colour sensations are met. It may be that the sensitive part of the retina is like a photographic plate, but with this essential difference—that the sensitive material is constantly changing. A photographic plate receives an impression which is not recognisable by the eye, though it can be shown that a change in the material does take place during the impact of light, by electrical and other means. When the eye receives an impression of light, Dewar has shown that in this case also a current of electricity is generated. Recent published experiments of my own have demonstrated that with a low intensity of light, the chemical change that occurs in a photographic salt is by no means proportionate to that which takes place with a greater intensity. In the eye, too, there is a limit of sensibility to very feeble light. Again, the curves of the stimulation of the colour sensations to the spectrum are closely of the same form as the curves200 of sensitiveness of the various sensitive salts used by photographers. These are analogies and, of course, must not be pressed too far. There must be such a complexity in the sensitive material in the eye, both chemical and physiological, that it may be that the changes induced by light on the sensitive surface of the retina have to be considered from both aspects. The purely chemical change is naturally that to which a physicist is most prone to incline, and his bias must be discounted, as must also that of the physiologist.


201

APPENDIX.

The following is extracted from Maxwell’s paper.

The following table contains the means of four sets of observations by the same observer (K.):—

Table IV. (K.)

44·3 (20) + 31·0 (44) + 27·7 (68) = W.
16·1 (28) + 25·6 (44) + 30·6 (68) = W.
22·0 (32) + 12·1 (44) + 30·6 (68) = W.
  6·4 (24) + 25·2 (36) + 31·3 (68) = W. 
15·3 (24) + 26·0 (40) + 30·7 (68) = W.
19·8 (24) + 35·0 (46) + 30·2 (68) = W.
21·2 (24) + 41·4 (48) + 27·0 (68) = W.
22·0 (24) + 62·0 (52) + 13·0 (68) = W.
21·7 (24) + 10·4 (44) + 61·7 (56) = W.
20·5 (24) + 23·7 (44) + 40·5 (60) = W.
19·7 (24) + 30·3 (44) + 33·7 (64) = W.
18·0 (24) + 31·2 (44) + 32·3 (72) = W.
17·5 (24) + 30·7 (44) + 44·0 (76) = W.
18·3 (24) + 33·2 (44) + 63·7 (80) = W.

X.—Reduction of the Observations.

By eliminating W from the equations above by means of the standard equation, we obtain equations202 involving each of the fourteen selected colours of the spectrum, along with the three standard colours; and by transposing the selected colour to one side of the equation, we obtain its value in terms of the three standards. If any of the terms of these equations are negative, the equation has no physical interpretation as it stands; but by transposing the negative term to the other side it becomes positive, and then the equation may be verified.

The following table contains the values of the fourteen selected tints in terms of the standards. To avoid repetition, the symbols of the standard colours are placed at the head of each column:—

Table VI.

Observer (K.) (24) (44) (68)
44·3 (20) = 18·6 +   0·4 +  2·8
16·1 (28) = 18·6 +   5·8  -   0·1
22·0 (32) = 18·6 + 19·3  -   0·1
25·2 (36) = 12·2 + 31·4  -   0·8
26·0 (40) =   3·3 + 31·4  -   0·2
35·0 (46) = - 1·2 + 31·4 +   0·3
41·4 (48) = - 2·6 + 31·4 +   3·5
62·0 (52) = - 3·4 + 31·4 + 17·5
61·7 (56) = - 3·1 + 21·0 + 30·5
40·5 (60) = - 1·9 +   7·7 + 30·5
33·7 (64) = - 1·1 +   1·1 + 30·5
32·3 (72) = + 0·6 +   0·2 + 30·5
44·0 (76) = + 1·1 +   0·7 + 30·5
63·7 (80) = + 0·3  -   1·8 + 30·5

203

Mr. James Simpson, formerly student of Natural Philosophy in my class, has furnished me with thirty-three observations taken in good sunlight. Ten of these were between the two standard colours, and give the following result:—

33·7 (88) + 33·1 (68) = W.

The mean errors of these observations were as follows:—

Error of (88) = 2·5; of (68) = 2·3; of (88) + (68) = 4·8; of (88) - (68) = 1·3.

The fact that the mean error of the sum was so much greater than the mean error of the difference, indicates that in this case, as in all others that I have examined, observations of equality of tint can be depended on much more than observations of equality of illumination or brightness.

From six observations of my own, made at the same time, I have deduced the “trichromic” equation—

22·6 (104) + 26 (88) + 37·4 (68) = W      (2)

If we suppose that the light which reached the organ of vision was the same in both cases, we may combine these equations by subtraction, and so find

22·6 (104) - 7·7 (88) + 4·3 (68) = D      (3)

204

where D is that colour, the absence of the sensation of which constitutes the defect of the dichromic eye.

The sensation which I have in addition to those of the dichromic eye is therefore similar to the full red (104), but different from it in that the red (104) has 7·7 of green (88) in it which must be removed, and 4·3 of blue (68) substituted. This agrees pretty well with the colour which Mr. PoleA describes as neutral to him, though crimson to others. It must be remembered, however, that different persons of ordinary vision require different proportions of the standard colours, probably owing to differences in the absorptive powers of the media of the eye, and that the above equation (2), if observed by K., would have been

23 (104) + 32 (88) + 31 (68) = W      (4)

and the value of D, as deduced from these observers, would have been

23 (104) - 1·7 (88) - 1·1 (68) = D      (5)

in which the defective sensation is much nearer to the red of the spectrum. It is probably a colour to which the extreme red of the spectrum tends, and which differs from the extreme red only in not containing that small proportion of “yellow” light which renders it visible to the colour blind.

A Philosophical Transactions, 1859, Part I., p. 329.

205

From other observations by Mr. Simpson the following results have been deduced:—

Table A.

(88) (68) (88) (68)
(99·2 +)   = 33·7   1·9 100 (96) = 108     7
31·3 (96) = 33·7   2·1 100 (92) = 120     5
28     (92) = 33·7   1·4 100 (88) = 100     0
33·7 (88) = 33·7    0     100 (84) =   61   11
54·7 (84) = 33·7   6·1 100 (82) =   47   21
71     (82) = 33·7 15·1 100 (80) =   34   33
99     (80) = 33·7 33·1 100 (78) =   22   47
70     (78) = 15·7 33·1 100 (76) =   10   59
56     (76) =   5·7 33·1 100 (72) =     1   92
36     (72) =   0·3 33·1 100 (68) =     0 100
33·1 (68) =    0     33·1 100 (64) =     0   83
40     (64) =   0·2 33·1 100 (60) =     3   60
55·5 (60) =   1·7 33·1
(57 -)       =   0·3 33·1

In the table on the left side (99·2 +) means the whole of the spectrum beyond (99·2) on the scale, and (57 -) means the whole beyond (57) on the scale. The position of the fixed lines with reference to the scale was as follows:—

A, 116; a, 112; B, 110; C, 106; D, 98·3; E, 88; F, 79; G, 61; H, 44.

The values of the standard colours in different parts of the spectrum are given on the right side of the above table, and are represented by the curves of206 Fig. 9, Plate II., where the left-hand curve represents the intensity of the “yellow” element, and the right-hand curve that of the “blue” element of colour as it appears to the colour blind.

The appearance of the spectrum to the colour blind is as follows:—

From A to E the colour is pure “yellow,” very faint up to D, and reaching a maximum between D and E. From E to one-third beyond F towards G the colour is mixed, varying from “yellow” to “blue,” and becoming neutral or “white” at a point near F. In this part of the spectrum the total intensity, as given by the dotted line, is decidedly less than on either side of it, and near the line F, the retina close to the “yellow spot” is less sensible to light than the parts further from the axis of the eye. This peculiarity of the light near F is even more marked in the colour blind than in the ordinary eye. Beyond F the “blue” element comes to a maximum between F and G, and then diminishes towards H, the spectrum from this maximum to the end being pure “blue.”

The results given above were all obtained with the light of white paper, placed in clear sunshine. I have obtained similar results when the sun was hidden, by using the light of uniformly illuminated clouds, but I207 do not consider these observations sufficiently free from disturbing circumstances to be employed in calculation. It is easy, however, by means of such observations, to verify the most remarkable phenomena of colour blindness, as, for instance, that the colours from red to green appear to differ only in brightness, and that the brightness may be made identical by changing the width of the slit; that the colour near F is a neutral tint, and that the eye in viewing it sees a dark spot in the direction of the axis of vision; that the colours beyond are all blue of different intensities, and that any “blue” may be combined with any “yellow” in such proportions as to form “white.” These results I have verified by the observations of another colour-blind gentleman, who did not obtain sunlight for his observations; and as I have now the means of carrying the requisite apparatus easily, I hope to meet with other colour-blind observers, and to obtain their observations under more favourable circumstances.

Measurements of Colour Fields.

Some experiments in the measurement of the colour fields in the horizontal direction with the pure spectrum colours will help to show what importance is to be attached208 to the luminosity of the colour and the size of the spot of light with which the observations are made. A yellow and a blue of the spectrum were taken of such hues that when mixed they formed a patch of white light similar to the electric light. Their luminosities were measured, and the yellow found to be 1·6 of the light of an amyl-acetate lamp or 1·28 standard candles; the blue was 1/24 of this luminosity. The fields for these two colours were measured by automatically throwing spots of each colour separately on a white card which moved round a centre over which the eye was placed. The light was subsequently diminished to ½, ¼, and ⅛ of the above values, and readings again made. The following results were obtained with a spot of ·7 inch diameter:—

Light Yellow. Blue.
Nasal side. Temporal side. Nasal side. Temporal side.
Full 33° 45° 35° 45°
½ 24° 36° 26° 38°
¼ 18° 24° 22° 32°
11° 15° 19° 30°

With a spot of ·3 inch diameter the following were obtained:—

Full 24° 32° 21° 27°
½ 17° 28° 16° 22°
¼ 13° 16° 14° 20°
  8° 10° 13° 16°

209

It will be evident how the field contracts as the light is diminished in brightness, and also that the blue field does not diminish equally with the yellow field, but is more persistent. Again, it will be noticed that the luminosity of the blue, for the same extent of field to be covered, has to be much lower than for the yellow.

The diminished area of the spot of light also diminishes the field, and the same order of diminution of field is obtained as with the larger spot.

Another set of experiments, made with the same aperture of slit passed through the spectrum, and the field taken at different points, give the following results:—

Spectrum scale.
(See Fig. 41, Page 210.
Nasal side. Temporal side.
58·6 18° 35°
54·6 27° 46°
50·6 33° 47°
46·6 25° 30°
42·6 21° 21°
38·6 17° 17°
34·6 22° 30°
30·6 25° 33°
26·6 33° 40°
22·6 37° 44°
18·6 28° 40°
14·6 22° 34°
  8·6 20° 30°

210

Here we see that although the luminosity of the colour spots varies at the spectrum luminosity, the fields do not vary proportionally; when the luminosities of the green, yellow and red are made equal, the fields become nearly equal on the nasal side. The field for the blue, however, then becomes vastly larger than that for the others, showing a peculiarity which is very remarkable.

Fig. 41.

Spectrum Scale.

Recently published experiments on colour fields have been so largely based on the exigencies of the Hering theory, that it is somewhat difficult to decide their significance from any other aspect.

211

Table I.—Luminosity Curves for the Normal Eye (see Fig. 20).

I. II. III. IV. V.
Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
Outside
yellow spot.
Yellow
spot.
Fovea
centralis.
64 7217
63 7082 ..     1      
62 6957   1          2           2    
61 6839   2          4           4    
60 6728   3·5       7           8    
59 6621   7·5     12·5     15·5
58 6520 12·5     21        24   
57 6423 19        33        37·5
56 6330 27·5     50        60   
55 6242 35        65        77   
54 6152 43        80        90   
53 6074 52·5     90        97   
52 5996 61·0     96      100   
51 5919 71·0     99      100   
50 5850 79·0   100        98   
49 5783 84        99        95   
48 5720 85        97        90   
47 5658 83·5     92·5     85   
46 5596 81·0     87        79   
45 5538 77·0     81        72·5
44 5481 72.5     75        66   
43 5427 68·0     69        59   
42 5373 62·5     62·5     51   
41 5321 57        57        45   
40 5270 52        50        40   
39 5221 46        42·5     32   
38 5172 41·5     36        27·5
37 5128 37·5     29·5     22·0
36 5085 33·5     24        18   
35 5043 30·0     18·2     14   
34 5002 26·5     14·2     10   
33 4963 24        10·5       8·4
32 4924 21          8·5       6·5
31 4885 18·5       7·0       5·5
30 4848 16·5       5·5       4·0
29 4812 14·5       4·7       3·5
28 4776 13·0       4·0       3·0
27 4742 11·5       3·5       2·0
26 4707 10·5       2·8       2·4
25 4675   9·4       2·3       2·1
24 4639   8·2       1·82     1·9
23 4608   7·3       1·6       1·5
22 4578   6·3       1·4  
21 4548   5·7       1·2  
20 4517   5·0       1·08     1·0
19 4488   4·5         ·94
18 4459   4·0         ·86
17 4437   3·6         ·78
16 4404   3·1         ·70
15 4377   2·7         ·62       ·62
14 4349   2·3         ·56
13 4323   2·1         ·50
12 4296   1·9         ·45
11 4271   1·65       ·40
10 4245   1·4         ·34
  9 4221   1·2         ·30
  8 4197   1·0         ·26
  7 4174     ·88       ·22
  6 4151     ·75       ·18
  5 4131     ·63       ·16
  4 4106     ·50       ·14

214

Tables II. and III.—Curves of Luminosity of a Partially Red-Blind and of a Partially Green-Blind Person (see Fig. 23).

Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
Luminosity.
Red-blind. Green-blind.
64 7217   0         1    
62 6957   1         2    
60 6728   2         7    
58 6520   6       21    
56 6330 12       50    
54 6152 26       80    
52 5996 49       96    
50 5850 70       98    
48 5720 77       93    
46 5596 77       83    
44 5481 70       70    
42 5373 61       55    
40 5270 47       40    
38 5172 34       27    
36 5085 23       18    
34 5002 14       10    
32 4924   8·5     5·5
30 4848   5·5     3·0
28 4776   4·0     2·5
26 4707   2·7     2·0
24 4639   1·8     1·8
22 4578   1·35   1·4
20 4517   1·1     1·1

215

Table IV.—Luminosity of Spectrum Reduced in Intensity, so that D = 1/132·5 Amyl Lamp 1 Foot Distant (see Fig. 25).

Scale
number.
Mean
reading.
Mean reading, reduced to 100 max. P. and Q.’s readings, 100 max. Persistency curve for the centre of the eye.
55·6 ·5     ·6   2   2
53·6 5·5     7·0      3·6      3·6
51·6 13 16·7   8   8
49·6 23 29·7 22 22
47·6 40 50·0 44 44
45·6 57 71·2 69 69
43·6 70 87·5 93 93
41·6 79 98·7 100      99·5
39·6 78 97·5    99·5    98·5
37·6 74 92·5 96 93
35·6 66 82·5 89 84
33·6 55 68·7    77·5 71
31·6    44·5 55·2 61    53·5
29·6 35 43·7    45·5    36·5
27·6 24 30·0    33·5 24
25·6 17 21·7 25 16
23·6 13 16·7 18 10
21·6 10 12·5 13   8
19·6   8 10·0      9·5   6
13·6   3   3·7      4·2   3
  9·6   2   2·5      2·5   2

216

Table V.—Limit of Colour Vision (see Fig. 26.).

Scale
Number.
Wave-
Length.
Mean reading of the colour limit of the spectrum D, being 1 amyl lamp in 1/10000ths. Luminosity of the ordinary spectrum. Luminosity of the rays when each colour disappears, each ray having the original luminosity of 1 amyl lamp in 1/100000ths.
61 6839 120     4   48·0
60 6728 67   7   46·9
58 6520 26   21     54·6
56 6330 13   50     65·0
54 6152    9·5 80     76·0
52 5996    9·0 96     86·4
50 5850    9·0 100       90·0
48 5720    9·0 97     87·3
44 5481    9·5 75     71·3
40 5270  10·5 50     52·5
36 5085  12·5 24     30·0
32 4924 18    8·5 15·3
28 4776 32    4·0 12·8
24 4639 55    1·8 12·0
20 4517 90      1·08   9·7
16 4404 160          ·70 11·2
12 4296 250          ·45 11·0
  8 4197 400          ·26 10·4
  4 4106 700          ·14   9·8

217

Table VI.—Extinction by Central Portion of Normal Eye (see Fig. 28).

I. II. III. IV. V. VI.
Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
E.
Reduction of original luminosity in millionths to cause extinction.
L.
Luminosity of original beam.
(E × L) / 100 Persistency curve 650 / E (Maximum = 100).
64 7217 55,000     
63 7082 30,000        1 300·0        
62 7957 15,000        2 300·0        
61 6839 7500     4 300·0        
60 6728 3750     7 262·5        
59 6621 1900      12·5 237·5                ·34
58 6520 1050   21 220·5                ·62
57 6423 650 33 214·5             1·0
56 6333 380 50 190·0               1·71
55 6242 272 65 176·8               2·38
54 6152 196 80 156·0               3·32
53 6074 140 90 126·0               4·64
52 5996   97 96 93·12           6·70
51 5919   57 99 56·43         11·40
50 5850   35 100   35·0         18·6
49 5783   24 99 23·76       27·1
48 5720   17 97 16·49       38·2
47 5658      12·6    92·5 11·65       51·6
46 5596      10·2 87 8·87     63·7
45 5538        8·6 81 6·97     75·6
44 5481        7·4 75 5·55     87·8
43 5427        6·7 69 4·62     97·0
42 5373          6·55    62·5 4·09     99·5
41 5321        6·5 57 3·705 100   
40 5270          6·55 50 3·27     98·5
39 5221          6·65    42·5 2·83     97·5
38 5172          6·85 36 2·46     95·0
37 5128        7·2    29·5 2·12     90·0
36 5085        7·6 24 1·82     81·3
35 5043          8·15    18·2 1·48     80·0
34 5002        8·8    14·2 1·25     74·0
33 4963      10·2    10·5 1·07     63·0
32 4924      11·6      8·5   ·988   56·0
31 4885      13·6      7·0   ·952   47·7
30 4848      16·3      5·5   ·896   40·0
29 4812      20·5      4·7   ·963   31·7
28 4776      26·0      4·0 1·040   25·0
27 4742      31·0      3·5 1·085   20·9
26 4707      38·5      2·8 1·078   16·9
25 4674      46·0      2·3 1·058   14·1
24 4639      56·0        1·82 1·019   11·6
23 4608      67·0      1·6 1·072     9·7
22 4578   80      1·4 1·120       8·41
21 4548   95      1·2 1·140       7·22
20 4517 107        1·08 1·156     6·1
19 4488 124          ·94 1·165       5·23
18 4459 140          ·86 1·204       4·64
17 4437 160          ·78 1·228     4·1
16 4404 180          ·70 1·260       3·60
15 4377 200          ·62 1·240       3·25
14 4349 220          ·56 1·232       2·95
13 4323 240          ·50 1·200     2·7
12 4296 270          ·45 1·215     2·4
11 4271 300          ·40 1·200       2·18
10 4245 335          ·34 1·139       1·94
  9 4221 375          ·30 1·125       1·73
  8 4197 430          ·26 1·118       1·51
  7 4174 490          ·22 1·078       1·32
  6 4151 510          ·18   ·918       1·27
  5 4131 640          ·16 1·024       1·01
  4 4106 750          ·14 1·050       0·86

220

Table VII.—Extinction by Whole Eye (see Fig. 28).

I. II. III. IV. V. VI.
Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
E.
Reduction of original luminosity in millionths to cause extinction.
L.
Luminosity of original beam.
(E × L) / 160 Persistency curve 650 / E (Maximum = 100).
38 5172   6·9 41·5 2·86 94·2
37 5128   7·1 37·5 2·66 91·6
36 5085   7·4 33·5 2·48 87·8
35 5043   7·7 30·0 2·31 84·4
34 5002   8·0 26·5 2·12 81·2
33 4963   8·4 24·0 2·02 77·5
32 4924   8·8 21·0 1·85 73·8
31 4885   9·4 18·5 1·74 69·2
30 4848 10·0 16·5 1·65 65·0
29 4812 10·7 14·5 1·55 60·6
28 4776 11·5 13·0 1·49 56·5
27 4742 13·0 11·5 1·49 50·0
26 4707 14·5 10·5 1·52 44·8
24 4639 18·5   8·2 1·52 34·1
22 4578 23·0   6·3 1·45 28·3
20 4517 30·0   5·0 1·50 21·7
18 4459 39·0   4·0 1·56 16·7
16 4404 51      3·1 1·59 12·3
14 4349 66      2·3 1·52     9·85
12 4296 80      1·9 1·52     8·12
10 4245 110        1·4 1·54     5·91
  8 4197 154        1·0 1·54     4·22
  6 4151 204            ·75 1·54     3·18
  4 4106 307          ·5 1·54     2·11
  2 4063 513          ·3 1·54     1·26
  0 4020 770          ·2 1·54       ·84

From 38 to 64 the extinction is the same as with the central part of the eye.

222

Table VIII.—P.’s CurvesB (see Fig. 31).

I. II. III. IV. V. VI. VII.
Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
Mean reading of extinction in millionths of original luminosity. Adopted reading in millionths of original luminosity. Persistency curve (680 / ad. reading). P.’s luminosity curve. Absolute luminosity of extinction. (IV. × VI.) / 14
52 5996 68 68 10   7 34   
50 5850 35 35    19·4 19 47·5
48 5720 17 17 40 39 47·3
46 5596    10·2 10 68 65 46·4
45 5538      9·3      9·0 76 76 48·8
44 5481      8·0      8·1 84 90 52·8
42 5373      7·2      7·2    94·5 98 50·3
40 5270      6·7      6·8 100   99 48·1
38 5172      7·2      7·0 97    97·5 48·7
36 5085        8·05      7·7 90 90 49·5
34 5002        8·05      8·4 81 80 47·9
32 4924      9·9      9·8 69 65 45·5
30 4848    13·2    12·5 54 50 44·6
28 4776    13·9    15·0    45·3 36 38·6
27 4742    16·8    17·0 40    31·5 38·2
26 4707    21·6    20·5 32    26·5 38·8
24 4639 30 27 25    19·5 37·6
22 4578 36 35 19 14 35   
20 4517 42 45    15·5 10 32·2
16 4404 79 79      8·5      5·5 31·2
10 4245 180   190        3·6      2·5 32·2
  6 4151 270   270        2·7

B In this and the next two Tables the intensity of the illumination of the D ray before reduction is equal to that of an amyl-acetate lamp at one foot from a screen. The figures in Col. VII. are in millionths of the illumination of an amyl-acetate lamp at one foot distant, every ray being made of that intensity.

223

Table IX.—H. R.’s Curves (see Fig. 32).

I. II. III. IV. V. VI. VII.
Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
Mean reading of extinction in millionths of original luminosity. Adopted reading in millionths of original luminosity. Persistency curve (590 / ad. reading). Luminosity curve. Absolute luminosity of extinction (IV. × VI.) / 48
57 6423 1200     1200        ·49 5   125
56 6330 900   850      ·69 7   124
55 6242 500   550    1·07 10     115
54 6152 250   250    2·36 17       88
53 6074 .. 150    3·93 25       78
52 5996 90 90  6·56 35       66
51 5919 60 45  13·1     47       44
50 5850 27 27  21·8     57       32
48 5720 18 15  39·3     66       21
46 5596 10 10 59       69       14
44 5481      9·3   8  73·8     64       11
42 5373      6·5      6·2  95·1      56·5       7
40 5270      5·9      5·9 100         45            5·5
38 5172   6   6  98·3     32         4
36 5085 ..      6·6  89·4     20            2·7
35 5043   7      7·2  81·9     16            2·4
34 5002 ..   8  73·8      12·5          2·1
32 4924 10      9·6  61·5     8          1·6
30 4848    11·5 12  49·2     6          1·5
28 4776    14·5    14·5  40·7     5          1·5
26 4707 20    17·5  33·7     4          1·5
24 4639 20 22  26·8     3          1·4
22 4578 .. 30  19·7        2·4          1·5
18 4459 55 57  10·4        1·3          1·5
14 4349 115   115    5·1        ·7          1·7
10 4245 .. 160    3·7        ·5          1·7
6 4151 200   200    2·9        ·4          1·7

224

Table X.—V. H.’s Curves (see Fig. 33).

I. II. III. IV. V. VI. VII.
Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
Mean reading of extinction in millionths of original luminosity. Adopted reading in millionths of original luminosity. Persistency curve (530 / ad. reading). Luminosity curve. Absolute luminosity of extinction (IV. × VI.) / 75.
57 6423 500     500        1·1 31       206      
56 6330 350     350        1·5 43       200      
54 6152 200     180        2·9 61       146·4   
52 5996 100     100        5·3 70        93·3  
50 5850   40       40      13·3 73        38·9  
48 5720     ..     25      21·2 69         23      
46 5596   10       10      53·0 63          8·4  
45 5538    6·5    6·5  81·6 58          5·0  
44 5481    6·0    5·7   93     54          4·1  
42 5373    5·5    5·3 100     46          3·3  
40 5270    5·5    5·4  98·2 36          2·6  
38 5172    5·7    5·7   93     24          1·8  
36 5085    6·7    6·5  81·6 15          1·3  
34 5002    7·0    7·0  75·7  9·5        ·89
32 4924    8·5    8·5  62·3  7·0        ·79
30 4848  10·7  10·5  50·5  5·0        ·70
28 4776   16       16      33·1  3·7        ·79
26 4707     ..    22·5  23·5  2·7        ·81
24 4639   30       31      17·1  1·82      ·75
22 4578  42·5   42      12·6  1·4        ·78
20 4517   55       55        9·6  1·0        ·73
16 4404 105     100        5·3  ·7      ·93
12 4296 175     170        3·1  ·45    1·02
10 4245 200     200        2·7  ·34      ·91

225

Table XI.—B. C.’s Curves (see Fig. 34).

I. II. III. IV. V. VI.
Scale
Number.
Wave-
length.
Adopted reading in hundred thousandths. Persistency curve 12,500 readings in V. Luminosity of original beam. Absolute luminosity of extinction III. and V.
61 6839 7500    1·6
60 6728 5500    2·3      ·5        27·5
59 6622 4000    3·1     40
58 6520 2800    4·5     56
57 6423 2000    6·2     80
56 6330 1500    8·3     90
55 6242 1150    10·8       92
54 6152 950  13·1    11·5      109·2
53 6074 750  16·6   16     120
52 5996 580  21·6    21·5   125
51 5919 430 29      28·5      122·5
50 5850 350 36     37        129·5
49 5783 275  45·5   47        129·2
48 5720 215 58     60     129
47 5658 170  73·4   76        129·2
46 5596 140  89·3   92     129
45 5538 125 100       98        122·5
44 5481 125 100       100       125
43 5427 130  96·1   97     126
42 5373 150 83     85        127·5
41 5321 180  69·4   65     117
40 5270 215 59     45          96·7
39 5221 250 50     30       75
38 5172 290 43        1·5      723·2
37 5128 335 37     16          53·6
36 5055 380 33      11·5        43·7
34 5002 500 25         35
32 4994 650 19         26
30 4848 850 14        2·5        23·3
28 4776 1100    11·4       22
26 4707 1500    8·3    1·5     22
24 4639 2000    6·2     20
22 4578 2700    4·6        13·5
18 4459 4750  
14 4349 7500  
10 4245 11000    

227

Table XII.—M.’s Luminosity Curve compared with the Normal (see Fig. 30).

I. II. III. IV. V. VI. VII.
Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
Mean
reading.
Mean
reading
× 1·8.
Normal luminosity curve, centre of eye. Difference of last two columns. Difference
× 5·15.
61 6839 2   3·6  4   ·4          2·57
59 6621 7   12·6    12·5  -·1             ·51
57 6423 18     32·4    33     +·6            3·09
55 6242 36     64·8    65     ·2         1·03
53 6074 49     88·2    89·5  1·3           6·71
52 5996 52     95·4    96·5  1·1           5·66
51 5919 54     97·2    99·5  2·3       11·8
50 5850 54     97·2    100       2·8       14·4
49 5782 52·5  94·5    99·5  5·0       25·7
48 5720 50     90       97     7·0       36·0
47 5658 46     82·8    92·5  9·7       49·9
46 5596 41     73·8    87     13·2         68·0
44 5481 32     57·6    75     17·4        89  
42 5373 23     43·2    62.5 19·3        99  
40 5270 17     30·6    50     19·4        100    
38 5172 10     17·5    35·5  18           93  
36 5085 4   7·2  24     16·8         86·5
34 5002  1·0 1·8  14·5  12·7         65·5
31 4885    ·5  ·7  6·5 5·8       37·7
28 4776 0   0     4   4          20·6

228

Table XIII.—Miss W.’s Curves (see Fig. 39).

Scale
number.
Wave-
length.
Readings. Extinction in 1/100000. Persistency
curve.
63 7082 0    
62 6957 1    
60 6728 7    
58 6520 18      
57 6423 28      
56 6330 43      
54 6152 76       900        2
52 5996 90       250        7
50 5850 95       130          13·5
48 5720 93       60     29
46 5596 83       34     51
44 5481 71       22     80
42 5321 58        18·5   92
40 5270 46        17·5   100  
38 5172 32       18     94
36 5085 21        19·5   90
34 5002  12·5     22     79
32 4924 7     27     65
30 4848  4·5   34     51
28 4776  3·0   40        38·5
25 4675  1·5   60     29
20 4518  0·4   250         7
19 4488  0·0   350         5
16 4404 600      

229

INDEX

PAGE
Absorption by the Yellow Spot 90
Artificial Spectrum 33
Cases of Defective Colour Vision unrecognised 67
Clerk Maxwell’s Colour-Box 42
Clerk Maxwell’s Colour Curves 47
Colour, and the Sensations required to produce it 50
Colour Blindness due to Disease 137
Colour-Blind Persons see a Grey in the Spectrum 65
Colour Discs 32
Colour Fields 13
Colour Matches made by the Colour Blind 70
Colour Patch Apparatus 18
Colour Patch Apparatus, Original Form of 19
Comparison of the Young and Hering Theory 189
Complex Colours matched by Simple Colours 22
Contrast Colours 187
Curious Case of Congenital Colour Blindness, A 164
Dalton Colour Blindness 58
Daltonism, or Colour Blindness 57
Defective Form Vision connected with Colour Deficiency due to Disease 138
Definition at different parts of the Retina 11
Enfeebled Spectrum Luminosity 98
Exhibiting Colour Blindness by Colour Discs 74
Extinction and Persistency Curves of Green-Blind Persons 127
Extinction and Persistency Curves of Monochromatic Vision 125
Extinction and Persistency Curves of Red-Blind Persons 127
Extinction of Colour 105230
Extinction of Light by the Centre and Periphery of the Eye 114
Extinction of Colour of equal Luminosity 110
Extinction of Light in the Spectrum 109
Eye: Explanation of its Functions 3
Fatigue of the Retina 6, 30
Field of View 10
Fovea Centralis 4
Fundamental Light 34
Green-Blind Person’s Description of the Spectrum, A 64
Green Monochromatic Vision 131
Helmholtz Diagram of Sensations 38
Heredity in Colour Blindness 58
Hering’s Colour Vision Theory 52
Hering’s Theory not tri-chromic 57
Holmgren’s Colour Tests 169
Kœnig’s Colour Sensation Curves 49
Lissajou’s Figures 37
Luminosity of the Spectrum to the Centre of the Eye, the Fovea Centralis, and outside the Yellow Spot 88
Luminosity of the Spectrum to partially Colour Blind 86
Luminosity of the Spectrum to the Colour Blind 81
Luminosity of the Spectrum to the Normal Eyed 78
Malingerers, Detection of 185
Matching Colours by Mixtures of Simple Colours 26
Maxwell’s Colour Equations 202
Maxwell’s Curves for Red Blindness 69
Measurement of Colour Fields 207
Monochromatic Vision and the Spectrum 66
Number of Cones in the Eye 8
Optograms 9
Pellet Tests 146
Pendulum Experiments 36
Persistency Curves 119
Primary Colours 25
Primary Pigment Colours 27
Progressive Atrophy of the Optic Nerve 153
Purkinje’s Figures 7
Purples 24
Red and Green matched 72
Red-Blind Person’s Description of the Spectrum, A 63
Retina, Structure of 6
Retinal Fatigue 6, 30
Rods and Cones 8
Seat of Visual Sensation 7231
Sensation Curves in Terms of Luminosity 93
Sensitiveness of the Eye 121
Simple Colours 17
Simulation of Red and Green Blindness 175
Spectrum described by the Tobacco Blind, The 143
Spectrum Test for Colour Blindness 181
Table of Wave-Lengths 17
Tables 211
Tobacco Ambyopia 140
Tobacco Blindness, Examples of 148
Violet Blindness 73
Visibility of an Object in light of different Colours 123
Visual Purple 9
White Monochromatic Vision 158
Wool Test, The 170
Yellow Spot 4
Yellow Spot and Colour Mixtures, The 28
Young’s Theory, Modification of 196

Works on Photography
BY
Capt. W. de W. ABNEY, C.B., D.C.L., F.R.S.,
Late Royal Engineers.


Instruction in Photography. Post 8vo., 3s. 6d.

Photography with Emulsions. Post 8vo., 3s.

Negative Making. Post 8vo., 1s.

Art and Practice of Silver Printing. Written in conjunction with Mr. H. P. Robinson. Post 8vo., 2s. 6d.

Platinotype. Written in conjunction with Lyonel Clarke. Post 8vo., 2s. 6d.


LONDON: SAMPSON LOW, MARSTON & COMPANY, LIMITED,
St. Dunstan’s House, Fetter Lane, Fleet Street, E.C.

LONDON: PRINTED BY WILLIAM CLOWES AND SONS, LIMITED,
STAMFORD STREET AND CHARING CROSS.

Transcriber’s Notes

Punctuation, hyphenation, and spelling were made consistent when a predominant preference was found in the original book; otherwise they were not changed.

Simple typographical errors were corrected; unbalanced quotation marks were remedied when the change was obvious, and otherwise left unbalanced.

Illustrations in this eBook have been positioned between paragraphs.

The index was not checked for proper alphabetization or correct page references.